Skip to main content
Log in

Detection of Sexual Orientation (“Gaydar”) by Homosexual and Heterosexual Women

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although there has been considerable research investigating the ability to identify sexual orientation from static images, or “gaydar,” few studies have considered the role of female sexual orientation or sexual interest (for example, sociosexual orientation) in judgment accuracy. In two studies, we investigated the sexuality detection ability, and masculinity and femininity as cues used in judgment. In Study 1, we recruited heterosexual (N = 55) and homosexual (N = 71) women to rate the sexual orientation of homosexual and heterosexual male and female targets (N = 80: 20 heterosexual men, 20 homosexual men, 20 heterosexual women, and 20 homosexual women). We found that detection accuracy was better than chance levels for both male and female targets and that male targets were more likely to be falsely labeled as homosexual than female targets were. Overall, female faces were more accurately identified as heterosexual or homosexual than male faces and homosexual female raters were biased towards labeling targets as homosexual. Sociosexuality did not influence the accuracy with which targets were identified as heterosexual or homosexual. In Study 2, 100 heterosexual and 20 homosexual women rated the stimulus for masculinity and femininity. Heterosexual women were rated as more feminine and less masculine than homosexual women and homosexual men were rated as more feminine and less masculine than heterosexual men. Sexual orientation of the judges did not affect the ratings. The results were discussed with a reference to evolutionary and cultural influences affecting sexual orientation judgment accuracy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Conner, B. (1999). Accuracy of judgments of sexual orientation from thin slices of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 538–547. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.538.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, G., Hank, L., Rauzi, T., & Simkins, L. (1987). Detection of sexual orientation by heterosexuals and homosexuals. Journal of Homosexuality, 13, 83–100. doi:10.1300/J082v13n04_05.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, M. J., Young, S. G., Brown, C. M., Sacco, D. F., & Claypool, H. M. (2008). Adaptive responses to social exclusion: Social rejection improves recognition of real and fake smiles. Psychological Science, 19, 981–983. doi:10.1111/j.14679280.2008.02187.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brinsmead-Stockham, K., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Macrae, N. (2008). Female sexual orientation and menstrual influences on person perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 729–734. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, L., & Gilroy, P. (2002). The role of appearance and nonverbal behaviors in the perception of sexual orientation among lesbians and gay men. Psychological Reports, 91, 115–122. doi:10.2466/pr0.2002.91.1.115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collier, K. L., Bos, M. W., & Sandfort, T. G. M. (2012). Intergroup contact, attitudes toward homosexuality, and the role of acceptance of gender non-conformity in young adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 899–907. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.12.010.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doyal, L., Paparini, S., & Anderson, J. (2008). ‘Elvis died and I was born’: Black African men negotiating same-sex desire in London. Sexualities, 11, 171–192. doi:10.1177/1363460707085469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. B., Johnson, K. L., Ambady, N., & Rule, R. O. (2010). Sexual orientation perception involves gendered facial cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1318–1331. doi:10.1177/0146167210378755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gutierres, S. E., Kenrick, D. T., & Partch, J. J. (1999). Beauty, dominance, and the mating game: Contrast effects in self-assessment reflect gender differences in mate selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1126–1134. doi:10.1177/01461672992512006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higham, P. A., Perfect, T. J., & Bruno, D. (2009). Investigating strength and frequency effects in recognition memory using type-2 signal detection theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 57–80. doi:10.1037/a0013865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, S. M., & Bremme, R. (2011). The effects of facial symmetry and sexually-dimorphic facial proportions on assessments of sexual orientation. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 5, 214–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. L., Gill, S., Reichman, V., & Tassinary, L. G. (2007). Swagger, sway, and sexuality: Judging sexual orientation from body motion and morphology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 321–334. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kite, M. E., & Deaux, K. (1987). Gender belief systems: Homosexuality and the implicit inversion theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linville, S. E. (1998). Acoustic correlates of perceived versus actual sexual orientation in men’s speech. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 50, 35–48. doi:10.1159/000021447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lübke, K. T., Hoenen, M., & Pause, B. M. (2012). Differential processing of social chemosignals obtained from potential partners in regards to gender and sexual orientation. Behavioral and Brain Research, 228, 375–387. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.018.

  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., Alnwick, K. A., Milne, A. B., & Schloerscheidt, A. M. (2002). Person perception across the menstrual cycle: Hormonal influences on social-cognitive functioning. Psychological Science, 13, 532–536. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., & DeWall, C. N. (2007). Adaptive attentional attunement: Evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 28–36. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholas, C. L. (2004). Gaydar: Eye-gaze as identity recognition among gay men and lesbians. Sexuality and Culture, 8, 60–86. doi:10.1007/s12119-004-1006-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penke, L., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Beyond global sociosexual orientations: A more differentiated look at sociosexuality and its effects on courtship and romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1113–1135. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rehnman, J., & Herlitz, A. (2006). Higher face recognition ability in girls: Magnified by own-sex and own-ethnicity bias. Memory, 14, 289–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rellecke, J., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2012). Does processing of emotional facial expressions depend on intention? Time-resolved evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biological Psychology, 90, 23–32. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G., Simmons, L., & Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 186–201. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieger, G., Linsemeier, J. A. W., Gygax, L., Garcia, S., & Bailey, J. M. (2010). Dissecting “gaydar”: Accuracy and the role of masculinity–femininity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 124–140. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9405-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roney, J. R., Hanson, K. N., Durante, K. M., & Maestripieri, D. (2006). Reading men’s faces: Women’s mate attractiveness judgments track men’s testosterone and interest in infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 273, 2169–2175. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). Brief exposures: Male sexual orientation is accurately perceived at 50 ms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1100–1105. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., & Hallett, K. C. (2009). Female sexual orientation is perceived accurately, rapidly, and automatically from the face and its features. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1245–1251. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.07.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N., Macrae, C. N., & Ambady, N. (2009). Ambiguous group membership is extracted automatically from faces. Psychological Science, 20, 441–443. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02314.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., Rosen, K. S., Slepian, M. L., & Ambady, N. (2011). Mating interest improves women’s accuracy in judging male sexual orientation. Psychological Science, 22, 881–886. doi:10.1177/0956797611412394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sacco, D. F., Hugenberg, K., & Sefcek, J. A. (2009). Sociosexuality and face perception: Unrestricted sexual orientation facilitates sensitivity to female facial cues. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 777–782. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samal, A., Subramani, V., & Marx, D. (2007). Analysis of sexual dimorphism in human face. Journal of Visual Communication and Image Representation, 18, 453–463. doi:10.1016/j.jvcir.2007.04.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 870–883. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.870.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, R., Jacobs, G., & Rogers, H. (2003). Male voices and perceived sexual orientation: An experimental and theoretical approach. Language in Society, 32, 329–350. doi:10.1017/S0047404503323024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 34–50. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sylva, D., Rieger, G., Linsemeier, J. A. W., & Bailey, J. M. (2010). Concealment of sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 141–152. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9466-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tabak, J. A., & Zayas, V. (2012). The roles of featural and configural face processing in snap judgments of sexual orientation. PLoS ONE, 7, e36671.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y.-J. (2012). Internet dating sites as heterotopias of gender performance: A case study of Taiwanese heterosexual male daters. International Journal of Cultural Studies, 15, 485–500. doi:10.1177/1367877911422855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woolery, L. M. (2007). Gaydar: A social-cognitive analysis. Journal of Homosexuality, 53, 9–17. doi:10.1300/J082v53n03_02.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, D. B., & Sladden, B. (2003). An own gender bias and the importance of hair in face recognition. Acta Psychologica, 114, 101–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Minna Lyons.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lyons, M., Lynch, A., Brewer, G. et al. Detection of Sexual Orientation (“Gaydar”) by Homosexual and Heterosexual Women. Arch Sex Behav 43, 345–352 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0144-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0144-7

Keywords

Navigation