Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Comparing responses of four ungulate species to playbacks of baboon alarm calls

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A growing body of evidence suggests that a wide range of animals can recognize and respond appropriately to calls produced by other species. Social learning has been implicated as a possible mechanism by which heterospecific call recognition might develop. To examine whether familiarity and/or shared vulnerability with the calling species might influence the ability of sympatric species to distinguish heterospecific alarm calls, we tested whether four ungulate species (impala: Aepyceros melampus; tsessebe: Damaliscus lunatus; zebra: Equus burchelli; wildebeest: Connochaetes taurinus) could distinguish baboon (Papio hamadryas ursinus) alarm calls from other loud baboon calls produced during intra-specific aggressive interactions (‘contest’ calls). Overall, subjects’ responses were stronger following playback of alarm calls than contest calls. Of the species tested, impala showed the strongest responses and the greatest difference in composite response scores, suggesting they were best able to differentiate call types. Compared with the other ungulate species, impala are the most frequent associates of baboons. Moreover, like baboons, they are susceptible to both lion and leopard attacks, whereas leopards rarely take the larger ungulates. Although it seems possible that high rates of association and/or shared vulnerability may influence impala’s greater ability to distinguish among baboon call types, our results point to a stronger influence of familiarity. Ours is the first study to compare such abilities among several community members with variable natural histories, and we discuss future experiments that would more systematically examine development of these skills in young ungulates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aubin T (1991) Why do distress calls evoke interspecific responses? An experimental study applied to some species of birds. Behav Process 23:103–111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Armitage KB (1997) Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots. I. The meaning of situationally variable alarm calls. Anim Behav 53:143–171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown GE, Adrian JC, Smyth E, Leet H, Brennan S (2000) Ostariophysan alarm pheromones: laboratory and field tests of the functional significance of nitrogen oxides. J Chem Ecol 26:139–154

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Busse C (1982) Leopard and lion predation upon chacma baboons living in the Moremi Wildlife Reserve. Botswana Notes Rec 12:15–21

    Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM (1995) Pursuit-deterrence revisited. Trends Ecol Evol 10:500–503

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM, Graham CM, Stoner CJ, Vargas JK (2004) Adaptive significance of antipredator behaviour in artiodactyls. Anim Behav 67:205–228. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2002.12.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Fischer J, Beehner J, Bergman T, Johnson SE, Kitchen DM, Palombit RA, Rendall D, Silk JB (2004) Factors affecting reproduction and mortality among baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. Int J Primatol 25:401–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Codron D, Codron J (2009) Reliability of δ13C and δ15N in faeces for reconstructing savanna herbivore diet. Mamm Biol 74:36–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Codron D, Lee-Thorp JA, Sponheimer M, de Ruiter D, Codron J (2006) Inter- and intrahabitat dietary variability of chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) in South African savannas based on fecal δ13C, δ15N, and %N. Am J Phys Anthropol 129:204–214

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Codron D, Codron J, Lee-Thorp JA, Sponheimer M, de Ruiter D, Sealy J, Grant R, Fourie N (2007) Diets of savanna ungulates from stable carbon isotope composition of faeces. J Zool 273:21–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coe R (2000) Effect size calculator. Durham University, Durham. Available via http://www.cemcentre.org/renderpage.asp?linkID=30325015. Accessed 9 Sept 2009

  • Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook M, Mineka S, Wolkenstein B, Laitsch K (1985) Observational conditioning of snake fear in unrelated rhesus monkeys. J Abnorm Psychol 94:591–610

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cowlishaw G (1994) Vulnerability to predation in baboon populations. Behaviour 131:293–304

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Kort SR, ten Cate C (2001) Response to interspecific vocalizations is affected by degree of phylogenetic relatedness in Streptopelia doves. Anim Behav 61:239–247. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1552

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellery WN, Ellery K, McCarthy TS (1993) Plant distribution in island of the Okavango Delta, Botswana: determinants and feedback interactions. Afr J Ecol 31:118–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes RD (1991) The behavior guide to African mammals. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C (2004) Reciprocal recognition of sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi) and redfronted lemur (Eulemur fulvus rufus) alarm calls. Anim Cogn 7:45–52. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0180-0

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2001) Acoustic features of female chacma baboon barks. Ethology 107:33–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2002) Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: influences of context, age, and individuality. J Acoust Soc Am 111:1465–1474

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff IR, Sundaresan SR, Cordingley J, Rubenstein DI (2007) Habitat use and movements of plains zebra (Equus burchelli) in response to predation danger from lions. Behav Ecol 18:725–729

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forsman JT, Monkkonen M (2001) Responses by breeding birds to heterospecific song and mobbing call playbacks under varying predation risk. Anim Behav 62:1067–1073. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1856

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin AS (2004) Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Learn Behav 32:131–140

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WJ, Buskirk RE, Buskirk WH (1976) Defense of space and resources by chacma (Papio ursinus) baboon troops in an African desert and swamp. Ecology 57:1264–1272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen RM, Mugambi MM, Bauni SM (1985) Diets and trophic ranking of ungulates of the northern Serengeti Kenya. J Wildl Manag 49:823–829

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson O (1991) Pursuit-deterrent signals- communication between prey and predator. Trends Ecol Evol 6:325–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (1988) How infant vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls: the role of experience. Behaviour 105:187–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayward MW, Kerley GIH (2008) Prey preferences and dietary overlap amongst Africa’s large predators. South Afr J Wildl Res 38:93–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedges L, Olkin I (1985) Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Holm S (1979) A simple sequential rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat 6:65–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson FR, McNaughton EJ, Shelley CP, Blumstein DT (2003) Mechanisms of heterospecific recognition in avian mobbing calls. Aust J Zool 51:577–585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jurisevic MA, Sanderson KJ (1998) A comparative analysis of distress call structure in Australian passerine and non-passerine species: influence of size and phylogeny. J Avian Biol 29:61–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kingdon J (1988) East African mammals: an atlas of evolution in Africa. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchen DM, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2003) Female baboons’ responses to male loud calls. Ethology 109:401–412

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lea AJ, Barrera JP, Tom LM, Blumstein DT (2008) Heterospecific eavesdropping in a nonsocial species. Behav Ecol 19:1041–1046. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn064

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann MB, Funston PJ, Owen CR, Slotow R (2008) Feeding behaviour of lions (Panthera leo) on a small reserve. South Afr J Wildl Res 38:66–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2007) A mutual understanding? Interspecific responses by birds to each other’s aerial alarm calls. Behav Ecol 18:944–951. doi:10.1093/beheco/arm063

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2009) Recognition of other species’ aerial alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? Proc R Soc B 276:769–774. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1368

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1957) Specific distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour 11:13–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathis A, Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1996) Cultural transmission of predator recognition in fishes: intraspecific and interspecific learning. Anim Behav 51:185–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCulloch CE, Searle SR (2001) Generalized, linear and mixed models. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald JH (2009) Handbook of biological statistics (2nd ed.). Sparky House Publishing, Baltimore. Available via http://udel.edu/~mcdonald/statintro.html. Accessed 18 Aug 2009

  • McGregor PK (1992) Quantifying responses to playback: one, many, or composite multivariate measures? In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum Press, New York, pp 79–96

    Google Scholar 

  • McGregor PK, Dabelsteen T (1996) Communication networks. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, pp 409–425

    Google Scholar 

  • Mineka S, Cook M (1993) Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. J Exp Psychol 122:23–38

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2001) Are chemical alarm cues conserved within salmonid fishes? J Chem Ecol 27:1641–1655

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller CA, Manser MB (2008) The information banded mongooses extract from heterospecific alarms. Anim Behav 75:897–904. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakagawa S, Cuthill IC (2007) Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists. Biol Rev 82:591–605

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newton PN (1989) Associations between langur monkeys (Presbytis entellus) and chital deer (Axis axis): chance encounter or a mutualism? Ethology 83:89–120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nocera JJ, Taylor PD, Ratcliffe LM (2008) Inspection of mob-calls as sources of predator information: response of migrant and resident birds in the Neotropics. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1769–1777. doi:10.1007/s00265-008-0605-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oda R (1998) The responses of Verreaux’s sifakas to anti-predator alarm calls given by sympatric ring-tailed lemurs. Folia Primatol 69:357–360. doi:10.1159/000021651

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oda R, Masataka N (1996) Interspecific responses of ringtailed lemurs to playback of antipredator alarm calls given by Verreaux’s sifakas. Ethology 102:441–453

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff FGT, DuToit JT (2004) Large predators and their prey in a southern African savanna: a predator’s size determines its prey size range. J Anim Ecol 73:410–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rainey HJ, Zuberbuhler K, Slater PJB (2004a) Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:755–759. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2619

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rainey HJ, Zuberbuhler K, Slater PJB (2004b) The responses of black-casqued hornbills to predator vocalisations and primate alarm calls. Behaviour 141:1263–1277. doi:10.1163/1568539042729658

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramakrishnan R, Coss RG (2000) Recognition of heterospecific alarm vocalizations by bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). J Comp Psychol 114:3–12. doi:10.1037//0735-7036.114.1.3

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Randler C (2006) Red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris) respond to alarm calls of Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius). Ethology 112:411–416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1986) The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species alarm calls. Anim Behav 34:754–764

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shriner WM (1998) Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 55:529–536

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shriner WM (1999) Antipredator response to a previously neutral sound by free-living adult golden-mantled ground squirrels, Spermophilus lateralis (Sciuridae). Ethology 105:747–757

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1981) Biometry. WH Freeman and Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • SYSTAT (2007) SYSTAT version 12.0. SPSS Incorporated, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E (2007) Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104:5479–5482

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood R (1982) Vigilance behavior in grazing African antelopes. Behaviour 79:8–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valone J (2007) From eavesdropping on performance to copying the behavior of others: a review of public information use. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1–14. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0439-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Bommel FPJ, Heitkönig IMA, Epema GF, Ringrose S, Bonyongo C, Veenendaal EM (2006) Remotely sensed habitat indicators for predicting distribution of impala (Aepyceros melampus) in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. J Trop Ecol 22:101–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitousek MN, Adelman JS, Gregory NC, St Clair JJH (2007) Heterospecific alarm call recognition in a non-vocal reptile. Biol Lett 3:632–634. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2007.0443

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Washburn SL, DeVore I (1961) The social life of baboons. Sci Am 204:62–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White C (1952) The use of ranks in a test of significance for comparing two treatments. Biometrics 8:37–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2000) Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 267:713–718

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Jenny D, Bishary R (1999) The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 105:477–490

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Office of the President of the Republic of Botswana and the Botswana Department of Wildlife and National Parks for permission to conduct this research. We thank J. Beehner, A. Mokupi, M. Mokupi, J. Rawle, C. McAllister and M. McAllister for support and assistance in the field, and four anonymous reviewers for suggestions. We are grateful to W. Smith, C. Shaw, I. Clark and the staff at Eagle Island Camp for use of vehicles and for friendship. Research was supported by National Science Foundation grant IBN 9514001, National Institute of Health Grant MH62249, The Leakey Foundation, the University of Pennsylvania, and The Ohio State University. This research adhered to the Animal Behavior Society Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research, the legal requirements of Republic of Botswana, and institutional guidelines.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dawn M. Kitchen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kitchen, D.M., Bergman, T.J., Cheney, D.L. et al. Comparing responses of four ungulate species to playbacks of baboon alarm calls. Anim Cogn 13, 861–870 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0334-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0334-9

Keywords

Navigation