Skip to main content
Log in

Within-group spatial position and vigilance: a role also for competition? The case of impalas (Aepyceros melampus) with a controlled food supply

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theory predicts that individuals at the periphery of a group should be at higher risk than their more central conspecifics since they would be the first to be encountered by an approaching terrestrial predator. As a result, it is expected that peripheral individuals display higher vigilance levels. However, the role of conspecifics in this “edge effect” may have been previously overlooked, and taking into account the possible role of within-group competition is needed. Vigilance behavior in relation to within-group spatial position was studied in impalas (Aepyceros melampus) feeding on standardized patches. We also controlled for food distribution in order to accurately define a “central” as opposed to a “peripheral” position. Our data clearly supported an edge effect, with peripheral individuals spending more time vigilant than their central conspecifics. Data on social interactions suggest that it was easier for a foraging individual to defend its feeding patch with its head lowered, and that more interactions occurred at the center of the group. Together, these results indicate that central foragers may reduce their vigilance rates in response to increased competition. Disentangling how the effects of competition and predation risk contribute to the edge effect requires further investigations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arenz CL, Leger DW (1999) Thirteen-lined ground squirrel (Sciuridae: Spermophilus tridecemlineatus) antipredator vigilance decreases as vigilance cost increases. Anim Behav 57:97–103

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balmford A, Turyaho M (1992) Predation risk and lek-breeding in Uganda kob. Anim Behav 44:117–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp G (2003) Group-size effects on vigilance: a search for mechanisms. Behav Proc 63:111–121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger J (1991) Pregnancy incentives, predation constraints and habitat shifts: experimental and field evidence for wild bighorn sheep. Anim Behav 41:61–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger J, Cunningham C (1988) Size-related effects on search times in North American grassland female ungulates. Ecology 69:177–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Daniel JC (2003) Red kangaroos (Macropus rufus) receive an antipredator benefit from aggregation. Acta Ethol 5:95–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Daniel JC, Ardron JG, Evans CS (2002) Does feeding competition influence tammar wallaby time allocation? Ethology 108:937–945

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger J, Gochfeld M (1994) Vigilance in African mammals: differences among mothers, other females, and males. Behaviour 131:153–169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger J, Safina C, Gochfeld M (2000) Factors affecting vigilance in springbok: importance of vegetative cover, location in herd, and herd size. Acta Ethol 2:97–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark CW, Mangel M (1986) The evolutionary advantages of group foraging. Theor Pop Biol 30:45–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elgar MA (1989) Predator vigilance and group size in mammals and birds: a critical review of the empirical evidence. Biol Rev 64:13–33

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Beauchamp G, Bastain B (2007) Group-size and distance-to-neighbour effects on feeding and vigilance in brown-headed cowbirds. Anim Behav 73:771–778

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortin D, Boyce MS, Merrill EH, Fryxell JM (2004) Foraging costs of vigilance in large mammalian herbivores. Oikos 107:172–180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frid A (1997) Vigilance by female Dall's sheep: interactions between predation risk factors. Anim Behav 53:799–808

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1971) Geometry of the selfish herd. J Theor Biol 31:295–311

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch BT (2007) Costs and benefits of within-group spatial position: a feeding competition model. Quart Rev Biol 82:9–27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inglis IR, Lazarus J (1981) Vigilance and flock size in brent geese: the edge effect. Z Tierpsychol 57:193–200

    Google Scholar 

  • Jennings T, Evans SM (1980) Influence of position in the flock and flock size on vigilance in the starling Sturnus vulgaris. Anim Behav 28:634–635

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause J (1994) Differential fitness returns in relation to spatial position in groups. Biol Rev 69:187–206

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lingle S (2001) Anti-predator strategies and grouping patterns in white-tailed deer and mule deer. Ethology 107:295–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mooring MS, Fitzpatrick TA, Nishihira TT, Reisig DD (2004) Vigilance, predation risk, and the Allee effect in desert Bighorn Sheep. J Wildl Manage 68:519–532

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro JC, Bates DM (2000) Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS, Springer, New York

  • Proctor CJ, Broom M, Ruxton GD (2006) Antipredator vigilance in birds: modelling the “edge” effect. Math Biosc 199:76–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulliam HR (1973) On the advantages of flocking. J Theor Biol 38:419–422

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team (2005) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. URL http://www.R-project.org.

  • Ron T, Henzi SP, Motro U (1996) Do female chacma baboons compete for a safe spatial position in a southern woodland habitat? Behaviour 133:475–490

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stankowich T (2003) Marginal predation methodologies and the importance of predator preferences. Anim Behav 66:589–599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood R (1982) Vigilance behaviour in grazing African antelopes. Behaviour 79:81–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwarts L (1976) Density-related processes in feeding dispersion and feeding activity of Teal (Anas crecca). Ardea 64:192–209

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

We are grateful to C. Bonenfant, S. Devillard, J.-M. Gaillard, M. Garel, M. Guillemain, M. Hewison, J. O’Brien and three anonymous referees for comments on an earlier draft. This project was developed within the HERD Project (CIRAD/CNRS). We are grateful to Zimbabwe Parks and Wildlife Management Authority for their support, and to the CNRS-NRF PICS program “Plant-herbivore dynamics in changing environments - developing appropriate models for adaptive management” for funding. Many thanks also to S. Le Bel (CIRAD-Zimbabwe) for facilitating the operations. We are also grateful to all the kids from Main Camp for their joyful support and pods collection, and we thank all the inhabitants of Main Camp for their understanding and tolerance as they adapted their routes during the observation sessions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pierrick Blanchard.

Additional information

Communicated by T. Czeschlik

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Blanchard, P., Sabatier, R. & Fritz, H. Within-group spatial position and vigilance: a role also for competition? The case of impalas (Aepyceros melampus) with a controlled food supply. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62, 1863–1868 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0615-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0615-3

Keywords

Navigation