Skip to main content
Log in

The Impact of Negative Affect on Performance Standards: Evidence for an Affect-as-Information Mechanism

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments investigated the influence of negative affect on self-regulatory cognition, including the mechanism by which negative affect may induce comparatively higher standards for performance. Both studies tested affect-as-information theory using a discounting manipulation. As predicted, induced negative affect generated comparatively higher minimal performance standards except under conditions in which the mood induction procedure was made highly salient. Negative affect had no impact on perceived self-efficacy. Results directly support the hypothesis that an affect-as-information mechanism underlies the impact of negative affect on performance standards. We discuss these findings both in terms of the affectively laden constructive processes through which people adopt standards for evaluating their actions and the role these processes may play in the perfectionism that is sometimes associated with depression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Ahrens, A. H. (1987). Theories of depression: The role of goals and the self-evaluation process. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 665–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alden, L. E., Bieling, P. J., & Wallace, S. T. (1994). Perfectionism in an interpersonal context: A selfregulation analysis of dysphoria and social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 297–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1017–1028.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1986). Differential engagement of self-reactive influences in cognitive motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38, 92–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1987). Cognitive model of depression. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1, 2–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blatt, S. J. (1995). The destructiveness of perfectionism: Implications for the treatment of depression. American Psychologist, 50(12), 1003–1020.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 31, 129–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1991). Mood congruity of social judgments. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments (pp. 31–53). Oxford, U.K.: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caprara, G. V., & Cervone, D. (2000). Personality: Determinants, dynamics, and potentials. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S. (1998). Generalization, adverse events, and development of depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality, 66(4), 607–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Ganellen, R. J. (1983). Depression and components of self-punitiveness: High standards, self-criticism, and overgeneralization. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 330–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. NewYork: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cervone, D., Kopp, D. A., Schaumann, L., & Scott, W. D. (1994). Mood, self-efficacy, and performance standards: Lower moods induce higher standards for performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 499–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cervone, D., & Peake, P. K. (1986). Anchoring, efficacy, and action: The influence of judgmental heuristics on self-efficacy judgments and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 492–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. In A. H. Hastorf & A.M. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology (pp. 73–108). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 25, 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S. I., Downey, G., & Schaffer-Neitz, R. (1999). Pain, negative mood, and perceived support in chronic pain patients: A daily study of people with reflex sympathetic dystrophy syndrome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 776–785.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (1991). On the task, the measures and the mood in research on affect and social cognition. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments (pp. 83–104). Oxford, U.K.: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., & Mittelstaedt, W. M. (1991). Dysphoria and components of self-punitiveness: A re-analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 201–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (1998). The persistent use of negative affect by anxious individuals to estimate risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1350–1363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S., & Terrell, F. (1977). Motivational and associative aspects of mild depression in skill and chance tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 389–401. ie]Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.). (1996). The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion and emotion regulation. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 525–552). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, P. L., & Dyck, D. G. (1986). Perfectionism, stress, and vulnerability to depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 137–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Dimensions of perfectionism in unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 98–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy theory: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., Levine, G. M., McDonald, H. E., Melton, R. J., & Martin, L. L. (1997). The role of mood in quantitative and qualitative aspects of performance: Single or multiple mechanisms? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(6), 602–629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 443–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Shalker, T., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E., Dougherty, F. E., Bloxom, B. M., & Kotsch, W. E. (1974). The differential emotions scale: A method of measuring the subjective experience of discrete emotions. Unpublished manuscript, Vanderbilt University.

  • Kanfer, F. H., & Zeiss, A. M. (1983). Depression, interpersonal standard-setting, and judgments of selfefficacy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 319–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Locke, K. D., & Audrain, P. C. (1993). The influence of attributions on the relevance of negative feelings to personal satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(1), 21–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keppel,G. (1991). Design and analysis:Aresearcher's handbook (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K, Dembo, T, Festinger, L, & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspiration. In J. M. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 333–378). New York: Ronald.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A, & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombardi, W. J, Higgens, E. T, & Bargh, J. A. (1987). The role of consciousness in priming effects on categorization: Assimilation versus contrast as a function of awareness of priming task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 411–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L, & Tesser,A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L, Ward, D. W, Achee, J. W, & Wyer, R. S. (1993). Mood as input: People have to interpret the motivational implications of their moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 317–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T, & Prentice, D. (1996). The construction of social norms and standards. In E. T. Higgens & A.W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic priniciples (pp. 799–829). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W, Cantor, N, & Feldman, S. (1996). Principles of self-regulation: The nature of willpower and self-control. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, L. S, & Uleman, J. S. (1990). Assimilation and contrast effects in spontaneous trait inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 224–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H. (1983). Nonverbal expressions and the accessibility of life experiene memories:Acongruency hypothesis. Social Cognition, 2, 62–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, T. B, Kuipers, N. A, & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677–688.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R, & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons in the analysis of variance. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. L. (1974). Relationship between goal discrepancy and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N, & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N, & Clore, G. L. (1988). How do I feel about it? Informative functions of affective states. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 44–62). Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. D. (1993). An approach to the study of discrete emotion: The effects of anger and sadness on evaluative judgments and personal standards. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. D. (1995, April). A test of the theoretical appraisal dimensions for anger and sadness. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Los Angeles.

  • Scott, W. D, & Ingram, R. E. (1998). Affective influences in depression: Conceptual issues, multiple mechanisms, and cognitive consequences. In W. F. Flack & J.D. Laird (Eds.), Emotions in psychopathology: Theory and research. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. D., Winters, R, & Beevers, C. G. (2000). Affective distress as a central and organizing symptom in depression: Psychological mechanisms. In S. L. Johnson, A. M. Hayes, T. M. Field, N. Schneiderman, & P. M. McCabe (Eds.), Stress, coping, and depression. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, D. (1992). Changes in the valence of the self as a function of mood. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior: Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 271–311). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, R. C, Mark, M. M, & Clore, G. L. (1994). Mood-related persuasion depends on (mis)attributions. Social Cognition, 12(4), 309–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E, & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J.D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tillema, J. L, Cervone, D, & Scott, W. D. (in press). Negative mood, perceived self-efficacy, and personal standards in dysphoria: The effects of contextual cues on self-defeating patterns of cognition. Cognitive Therapy and Research.

  • Wilson, T. D, & Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 37–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J, & Mischel, W. (1982). Influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 901–914.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zanna, M. P, & Rempel, J. K. (1988). Attitudes: A new look at an old concept. In D. Bar-Tal & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), The social psychology of knowledge (pp. 315–334). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Scott, W.D., Cervone, D. The Impact of Negative Affect on Performance Standards: Evidence for an Affect-as-Information Mechanism. Cognitive Therapy and Research 26, 19–37 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013837520031

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013837520031

Navigation