Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-06T21:48:13.141Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stereotyped responding by schizophrenic patients on a two-choice guessing task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Christopher D. Frith*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Clinical Research Centre, Harrow, Middlesex
D. John Done
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Clinical Research Centre, Harrow, Middlesex
*
1 Address for correspondence: Dr C. D. Frith, Division of Psychiatry, Clinical Research Centre, Watford Road, Harrow, Middlesex HAl 3UJ.

Synopsis

On each of a sequence of trials subjects had to guess whether a cross would appear on the left or the right side of a computer screen. The sequence of cross positions was random. Normal controls, manic-depressive patients and patients with senile dementia produced relatively random sequences of responses, as did acute schizophrenic patients with positive symptoms. Acute schizophrenic patients with negative symptoms and chronic patients produced more stereotyped sequences with many response alternations (LRLR). Chronic schizophrenic patients with negative symptoms and intellectual deterioration (defect state) produced very stereotyped sequences with many preservations (LLLL). This severe restriction of response sequences is similar to that shown by animals after treatment with amphetamine. It is suggested that it is due to an impairment of a higher order control process which normally inhibits the repetition of sequences of behaviour when these have proved inappropriate.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Armitage, S. G., Brown, C. R. & Denny, M. R. (1964). Stereotypy of response in schizophrenics. Journal of Clinical Psychology 20, 225230.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Attneave, F. (1959). Applications of Information Theory to Psychology. Holt-Dryden: New York.Google Scholar
Berkson, G. (1967). Abnormal stereotyped motor acts. In Comparative Psychopathology (ed. Zubin, J. and Hunt, H. F.), pp. 7694. Grune and Stratton: New York.Google Scholar
Bleuler, E. (1950). Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias (transl. Zinkin, J.). International Universities Press: New York.Google Scholar
Calloway, E. & Naghdi, J. (1982). An information processing model for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 34, 339347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crane, G. E. (1968). Tardive dyskinesia in patients treated with major neuroleptics: a review of the literature. American Journal of Psychiatry 124, 4048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cromwell, R. L. (1968). Stimulus redundancy and schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 146 (5), 360375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T. J. (1980). Molecular pathology of schizophrenia: more than one disease process? British Medical Journal 280, 6668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, F. J. (1978). Monitoring attention deployment by random numbers generation: An index to measure subjective randomness. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12, 3538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, A., Winokur, G. & Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Archives of General Psychiatry 26, 5763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. (1981). Schizophrenia: an abnormality of consciousness? In Aspects of Consciousness, Vol. 2 (ed. Underwood, G. and Stevens, R.), pp. 149168. Academic Press: London.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1970). Studies in pattern detection in normal and autistic children: II. Reproduction and production of colour sequences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 10, 120135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (1972). Cognitive mechanisms in autism: experiments with colour and tone sequence production. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia 2, 160173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guy, W. (1976). ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology, pp. 534537. Department of Health, Education and Welfare: Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
Hemsley, D. R. (1977). What have cognitive deficits to do with schizophrenic symptoms? British Journal of Psychiatry 130, 167173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horne, L. H., Evans, F. J. & Orne, M. T. (1982). Random number generation, psychopathology, and therapeutic changes. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 680683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbance of affective contact. Nervous Child 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1919). Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia (transl. Barclay, R. M.). R. E. Krieger: New York. (Reprinted 1971).Google Scholar
Krawiecka, M., Goldberg, D. & Vaughan, M. (1977). A standardised psychiatric assessment for rating chronic psychotic patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 55, 299308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, D. G. C., Johnstone, E. C. & Frith, C. D. (1982).Spontaneous involuntary disorders of movement in neuroleptic treated and untreated chronic schizophrenics – prevalence, severity and distributions. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 452461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, R. W., Caird, W. K. & Laverty, S. G. (1964). Overinclusive thinking and delusions in schizophrenic patients. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 68, 562566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridley, R. M. & Baker, H. F. (1982). Stereotypy in monkeys and humans. Psychological Medicine 12, 6172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridley, R. M., Baker, H. F. & Scraggs, P. R. (1979). The time course of the behavioural effects of amphetamine and their reversal by haloperidol in a primate species. Biological Psychiatry 14, 753765.Google Scholar
Ridley, R. M., Baker, H. F. & Haystead, T. (1981 a). Perseverative behaviour after amphetamine: dissociation of response tendency from reward association. Psychopharmacology 75, 283286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridley, R. M., Haystead, T. A. J. & Baker, H. F. (1981 b). An involvement of dopamine in higher order choice mechanisms in the monkey. Psychopharmacology 72, 173177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, T. W. (1982). Stereotypies: addictions or fragmented actions? Bulletin of the British Psychology Society 35. 297300.Google Scholar
Robbins, T. W. & Sahakian, B. J. (1981). Behavioural and neurochemical determinants of stereotypy induced by drugs. In Metabolic Disorders of the Nervous System (ed. Rose, F. C.), pp. 245291. Pitman:London.Google Scholar
Rylander, G. (1972). Psychoses and the punding and choreiform syndromes in addiction to central stimulant drugs. Folia Psychiatrica, Neurologica et Neurochirurgica 75, 203212.Google ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing. Psychological Review 81, 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1982). Reinforcement-induced behavioural stereotypy: how not to teach people to discover rates. Journal of Experimental Psychology and Genetics 111, 2359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. & Sartorius, N. (1974). The Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.Google Scholar
Withers, E. & Hinton, J. (1971). Three forms of the clinical test of the sensorium and their reliability. British Journal of Psychiatry 119,18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yacorzinski, G. K. (1941). Perceptual principles involved in the disintegration of a configuration formed in predicting the occurrence of patterns selected by chance. Journal of Experimental Psychology 29, 401406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar