Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 464
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
December 2014
Print publication year:
2014
Online ISBN:
9781139814744

Book description

Learning by Expanding challenges traditional theories that consider learning to be a process of acquisition and reorganization of cognitive structures within the closed boundaries of specific tasks or problems. Yrjö Engeström argues that this type of learning increasingly fails to meet the challenges of complex social change and fails to create novel artifacts and ways of life. In response, he presents an innovative theory of expansive learning activity, offering a foundation for understanding and designing learning as a transformation of human activities and organizations. The second edition of this seminal text features a substantive new introduction that illustrates the development and implementation of Engeström's theory since its inception.

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

References

Alt, R. (1975). Erziehung und Gesellschaft. Berlin: Volk und Wissen (in German).
Altshuller, G. S. (1984). Creativity as an exact science: The theory of the solution of inventive problems. New York: Gordon and Breach.
Anthony, G., Hunter, R. & Thompson, Z. (2014). Expansive learning: Lessons from one teacher’s learning journey. ZDM: The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 46(2).
Argyris, C. & Schön, D. A. (1974). Theory in practice: Increasing professional effectiveness. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Argyris, C. and Schon, D. A. (1978). Organizational learning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Arlin, P. K. (1984). Adolescent and adult thought: A structural interpretation. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger (pp. 258–271).
Arsen’ev, A. S., Bibler, V. S. & Kedrov, B. M. (1967). Analiz razvivajuchegosja poniatia. Moskva: Nauka (in Russian).
Atlas, J. A. (1985). A re-opening of the question of primitive mentality. New Ideas in Psychology, 3, 335–339.
Bakhtin, M. M. (1973). Problems of Dostoyevsky’s poetics. Ann Arbor, MI: Ardis.
Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic imagination: Four essays by M. M. Bakhtin. Edited by M. Holquist. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Bakhtin, M. M. (1982). The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Bakhurst, D. (1991). Consciousness and revolution in Soviet philosophy: From the Bolsheviks to Evald Ilyenkov. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bakhurst, D. (2009). Reflections on activity theory. Educational Review, 61(2), 197–210.
Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W. & Nesselroade, J. R. (1977). Life-span developmental psychology: Introduction to research methods. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Bartlett, F. (1941). Fatigue following highly skilled work. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 131, 247–257.
Bartlett, F. (1958). Thinking: An experimental and social study. London: George Allen & Unwin.
Basseches, M. (1984). Dialectical thinking and adult development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine Books.
Bateson, G. (1978). The birth of a matrix or double bind and epistemology. In M. M. Berger (Ed.), Beyond the double bind. New York: Brunner/Mazel (pp. 39–64).
Bereiter, C. (1985). Toward a solution of the learning paradox. Review of Educational Research, 55, 201–226.
Berger, M. M. (Ed.) (1978). Beyond the double bind. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General system theory: Foundations, development, applications. New York: Braziller.
Bibler, V. S. (1983–84). Thinking as creation (Introduction to the logic of mental dialogue). Soviet Psychology, XXII(1), 33–54.
Bibler, W. S. (1970). Hegel, Marx und das Problem der Wandlungen des logischen Aufbaus der wissenschaftlichen Tätigkeit. In T. I. Oiserman (Hrg.), Karl Marx und die moderne Philosophie. Moskau: Progress (S. 129–168; in German).
Bloomfield, B. P. (1986). Modeling the world: The social constructions of systems analysts. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Blunden, A. (2010). An interdisciplinary theory of activity. Chicago: Haymarket Books.
Bohm, D. (1980). Wholeness and the implicate order. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Bolgar, R. R. (1969). The training of elites in Greek education. In R. Wilkinson (Ed.), Governing elites: Studies in training and selection. New York: Oxford University Press (pp. 23–49).
Borras, Jr., S. M. (2008). La Vía Campesina and its global campaign for agrarian reform. Journal of Agrarian Change, 8(2–3), 258–289.
Bratus, B. S. (1986). The place of fine literature in the development of a scientific psychology of personality. Soviet Psychology, XXV(2), 91–103.
Bratus, B. S. & Lishin, O. V. (1983). Laws of the development of activity and problems in the psychological and pedagogical shaping of the personality. Soviet Psychology, XXI(1), 38–50.
Braverman, H. (1974). Labor and monopoly capital: The degradation of work in the twentieth century. New York: Monthly Review Press.
Brehmer, B. (1980). In one word: Not from experience. Acta Psychologica, 45, 223–241.
Broadbent, D. E. (1977). Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 181–201.
Brödner, P. (1985). Skill-based production – the superior concept to the “unmanned factory.” Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Production Research: Towards the Factory of the Future. Stuttgart, West Germany, August 20–22, 1985.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–532.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1983). The context of development and the development of context. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Historical and philosophical perspectives. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 147–184).
Bronkhorst, L. H., Meijer, P., Koster, B., Akkerman, S. F. & Vermunt, J. D. (2013). Consequential research designs in research on teacher education. Teaching and Teacher Education, 33, 90–99.
Bronowski, J. (1978). The visionary eye: Essays in the arts, literature, and science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Broughton, J. M. (1984). Not beyond formal operations but beyond Piaget. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger (pp. 395–412).
Brown, A. L. (1978). Knowing when, where, and how to remember: A problem of metacognition. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology. Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brown, A. L. (1982). Learning and development: The problems of compatibility, access and induction. Human Development, 25, 89–115.
Brown, A. L. (1992). Design experiments: Theoretical and methodological challenges in creating complex interventions in classroom settings. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2(2), 141–178.
Brown, A. L. & DeLoache, J. S. (1978). Skills, plans, and self-regulation. In R. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops?Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brown, A. L., Campione, J. C. & Day, J. D. (1981). Learning to learn: On training students to learn from texts. Educational Researcher, 10, 14–21.
Brown, A. L. & French, L. A. (1979). The zone of proximal development: Implications for intelligence testing in the year 2000. Intelligence, 3, 255–277.
Bruner, J. S. (1962). Introduction. In L. S. Vygotsky, Thought and language. Cambridge: The MIT Press (pp. v–x).
Bruner, J. S. (1974). Going beyond the information given. In J. S. Bruner, Beyond the information given. London: George Allen & Unwin (pp. 218–238).
Bruner, J. S. (1976). Nature and uses of immaturity. In J. S. Bruner, A. Jolly & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution. New York: Basic Books (pp. 28–64).
Bruner, J. S. (1985a). On teaching thinking: An afterthought. In S. F. Chipman, J. W. Segal & R. Glaser (Eds.), Thinking and learning skills. Vol. 2. Research and open questions. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 597–608).
Bruner, J. S. (1985b). Vygotsky: A historical and conceptual perspective. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 21–34).
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bullowa, M. (Ed.) (1979). Before speech: The beginnings of human communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bulmer, R. (1967). Why is the cassowary not a bird? A problem of zoological taxonomy among the Karam of the New Guinea highlands. Man, 2, 5–25.
Bunn, J. H. (1981). The dimensionality of signs, tools and models. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Buss, A. R. (1979). Dialectics, history, and development: The historical roots of the individual-society dialectic. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior. Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press (pp. 313–333).
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Cazden, C. B. (1981). Performance before competence: Assistance to child discourse in the zone of proximal development. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 3(1), 5–8.
Chaiklin, S. (1985). Beyond inferencing: Some cognitive processes that affect the direction of student reasoning in applying physical-science beliefs. Paper presented at the conference “Cognitive Processes in Student Learning,” Lancaster, England, July 18–21, 1985.
Cherns, A. (1980). Work and work organizations in the age of the microprocessor. In K. D. Duncan, M. M. Gruneberg & D. Wallis (Eds.), Changes in working life. Chichester: Wiley (pp. 253–268).
Cole, M. (1983). Introduction to V. V. Davydov & A. K. Markova, A concept of educational activity for schoolchildren. Soviet Psychology, XXI(2), 50–51.
Cole, M. (1985). The zone of proximal development: Where culture and cognition create each other. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 146–161).
Cole, M. (1986). Cross-cultural research in the socio-historical tradition. Paper presented at the First International Congress on Activity Theory, West Berlin, October 3–5, 1986.
Cole, M. (1988). Cross-cultural research in the sociohistorical tradition. Human Development, 31, 137–151.
Cole, M. & Griffin, P. (1980). Cultural amplifiers reconsidered. In D. R. Olson (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought. New York: Norton (pp. 343–364).
Cole, M. & Scribner, S. (1978). Introduction. In L. S. Vygotsky, Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (pp. 1–14).
Commons, M. L., Richards, F. A. & Armon, C. (Eds.) (1984). Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger.
Coulmas, F. & Ehlich, K. (Eds.) (1982). Writing in focus. The Hague: Mouton.
Cuban, L. (1984). How teachers taught. New York: Longman.
Cussins, A. (1992). Content, embodiment and objectivity: The theory of cognitive trails. Mind, 101, 651–688.
Damerow, P. (1980). Handlung und Erkenntnis in der genetischen Erkenntnistheorie Piagets und in der Hegelschen “Logik.” In H. Furth (Ed.), Arbeit und Reflexion. Köln: Pahl-Rugenstein (pp. 159–187; in German).
Damerow, P., Furth, P., Heidtmann, B. & Lefèvre, W. (1980). Probleme der materialistischen Dialektik. In P. Furth (Ed.), Arbeit und Reflexion. Köln: Pahl-Rugenstein (pp. 234–282; in German).
Davis, L. E. (1980). Changes in work environments: The next 20 years. In K. D. Duncan, M. M. Gruneberg & D. Wallis (Eds.), Changes in working life. Chichester: Wiley (pp. 197–216).
Davydov, V. V. (1982). The psychological structure and contents of the learning activity in school children. In R. Glaser & J. Lompscher (Eds.), Cognitive and motivational aspects of instruction. Berlin: Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften (pp. 37–44).
Davydov, V. V. (1990). Types of generalization in instruction: Logical and psychological problems in the structuring of school curricula. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Davydov, V. V. (2008). Problems of developmental instruction: A theoretical and experimental psychological study. New York: Nova.
Davydov, V. V. & Radzikhovskii, L. A. (1985). Vygotsky’s theory and the activity-oriented approach in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 35–65).
Davydov, V. V. & Zinchenko, V. P. (1982). The principle of development in psychology. Soviet Psychology, XX(1), 22–45.
Davydov, V. V., Markova, A. K. & Shumilin, E. A. (1980). Psihologicheskie problemy formirovanija potrebnosti i motivov uchebnoi dejatel’nosti. In Morivy uchebnoi i obschestvenno poleznoi dejatelnosti skolnikov i studentov. Moskva: MOPI (pp. 3–23; in Russian).
Dawydow, W. W. (1977). Arten der Verallgemeinerung im Unterricht. Berlin: Volk und Wissen (in German).
Dawydow, W. W., Lompscher, J. & Markowa, A. K. (1982). Ausbildung der Lerntätigkeit bei Schülern. Berlin: Volk und Wissen (in German).
Day, J. D. (1983). The zone of proximal development. In M. Pressley & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Cognitive strategy research: Psychological foundations. New York: Springer (pp. 155–175).
De Kleer, J. & Brown, J. S. (1983). Assumptions and ambiguities in mechanistic mental models. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 155–190).
Dell, P. F. (1980). Researching the family theories of schizophrenia: An exercise in epistemological confusion. Family Process, 19, 321–335.
Dell, P. F. (1982). Beyond homeostasis: Toward a concept of coherence. Family Process, 21, 21–41.
Dewey, J. (1910). How we think. Boston: Heath.
Dougherty, J. W. D. & Keller, C. M. (1985). Taskonomy: A practical approach to knowledge structures. In J. W. D. Dougherty (Ed.), Directions in cognitive anthropology. Urbana: University of Illinois Press (pp. 161–174).
Dreyfus, H. & Dreyfus, S. (1986). Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. New York: The Free Press.
Edwards, D. & Middleton, D. (1986). Conversation with Bartlett. Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 8, 79–89.
Eisenstein, E. (1985). On the printing press as an agent of change. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 19–33).
El’konin, D. B. (1977). Toward the problem of stages in the mental development of the child. In M. Cole (Ed.), Soviet developmental psychology. White Plains, NY: Sharpe (pp. 538–563).
Eliot, T. S. (1950). Introduction. In M. Twain, The adventures of Huckleberry Finn. London: The Cresset Press (pp. vii–xvi).
Engels, F. (1975). Anti-Dühring. Moscow: Progress.
Engels, F. (1976). Ludwig Feuerbach and the end of classical German philosophy. In K. Marx & F. Engels, Selected works in three volumes. Vol. 3. Moscow: Progress (pp. 335–376).
Engelsted, N., Hedegaard, M., Karpatschof, B. & Mortensen, A. (Eds.) (1993). The societal subject. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.
Engeström, R. (1995). Voice as communicative action. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 2, 192–214.
Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding: An activity-theoretical approach to developmental research. Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit.
Engeström, Y. (1989). The cultural-historical theory of activity and the study of political repression. International Journal of Mental Health, 17(4), 29–41.
Engeström, Y. (1989). The cultural-historical theory of activity and the study of political repression. International Journal of Mental Health, 17(4), 29–41.
Engeström, Y. (1991a). Activity theory and individual and social transformation. Multidisciplinary Newsletter for Activity Theory, No. 7/8, 14–15.
Engeström, Y. (1991b). Developmental work research: A paradigm in practice. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 13, 79–80.
Engeström, Y. (1991c). Developmental work research: Reconstructing expertise through expansive learning. In M. I. Nurminen & G. R. S. Weir (Eds.), Human jobs and computer interfaces. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Engeström, Y. (1991d). Non scolae sed vitae discimus: Toward overcoming the encapsulation of school learning. Learning and Instruction, 1, 243–259.
Engeström, Y. (1993). Developmental studies of work as a testbench of activity theory: Analyzing the work of general practitioners. In S. Chaiklin & J. Lave (Eds.), Understanding practice: Perspectives on activity and context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y. (1994). The working health center project: Materializing zones of proximal development in a network of organizational learning. In T. Kauppinen & M. Lahtonen (Eds.) Action research in Finland. Helsinki: Ministry of Labour.
Engeström, Y. (1995). Innovative organizational learning in medical and legal settings. In L. M. W. Martin, K. Nelson & E. Tobach (Eds.), Sociocultural psychology: Theory and practice of doing and knowing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y. (1996a). Development as breaking away and opening up: A challenge to Vygotsky and Piaget. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 55, 126–132.
Engeström, Y. (1996a). Developmental work research as educational research: Looking ten years back and into the zone of proximal development. Nordisk Pedagogik/Journal of Nordic Educational Research, 16, 131–143.
Engeström, Y. (1996b). Interobjectivity, ideality, and dialectics. Mind, Culture and Activity, 3, 259–265.
Engeström, Y. (1996c). The tensions of judging: Handling cases of driving under the influence of alcohol in Finland and California. In Y. Engeström & D. Middleton (Eds.), Cognition and communication at work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 199–232).
Engeström, Y. (1999a). Activity theory and individual and social transformation. In Y. Engeström, R. Miettinen & R-L. Punamäki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y. (1999b). Expansive visibilization of work: An activity-theoretical perspective. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 8, 63–93.
Engeström, Y. (1999c). Innovative learning in work teams: Analyzing cycles of knowledge creation in practice. In Y. Engeström, R. Miettinen & R-L. Punamäki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 377–404).
Engeström, Y. (2000a). Activity theory as a framework for analyzing and redesigning work. Ergonomics, 43(7), 960–974.
Engeström, Y. (2000b). From individual action to collective activity and back: Developmental work research as an interventionist methodology. In P. Luff, J. Hindmarsh & C. Heath (Eds.), Workplace studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y. (2003). The horizontal dimension of expansive learning: Weaving a texture of cognitive trails in the terrain of health care in Helsinki. In F. Achtenhagen & E. G. John (Eds.), Milestones of vocational and occupational education and training. Vol. 1. The teaching-learning perspective. Bielefeld: Bertelsmann.
Engeström, Y. (2005a). Developmental work research: Expanding activity theory in practice. Berlin: Lehmanns Media.
Engeström, Y. (2005b). Knotworking to create collaborative intentionality capital in fluid organizational fields. In M. M. Beyerlein, S. T. Beyerlein & F. A. Kennedy (Eds.), Collaborative capital: Creating intangible value. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Engeström, Y. (2007a). Enriching the theory of expansive learning: Lessons from journeys toward coconfiguration. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 14(1–2), 23–39.
Engeström, Y. (2007b). Putting Vygotsky to work: The Change Laboratory as an application of double stimulation. In H. Daniels, M. Cole & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 363–382).
Engeström, Y. (2008). From teams to knots: Activity-theoretical studies of collaboration and learning at work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y. (2009a). The future of activity theory: A rough draft. In A. Sannino, H. Daniels & K. D. Gutiérrez (Eds.), Learning and expanding with activity theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 303–328).
Engeström, Y. (2009b). Wildfire activities: New patterns of mobility and learning. International Journal of Mobile and Blended Learning, 1(2), 1–18.
Engeström, Y. (2011). From design experiments to formative interventions. Theory and Psychology, 21(5), 598–628.
Engeström, Y. & Blackler, F. (2005). On the life of the object. Organization, 12, 307–330.
Engeström, Y. & Engeström, R. (1986). Developmental work research: The approach and the application of cleaning work. Nordisk Pedagogik, 6, 2–15.
Engeström, Y. & Escalante, V. (1996). Mundane tool or object of affection? The rise and fall of the Postal Buddy. In B. A. Nardi (Ed.), Context and consciousness: Activity theory and human-computer interaction. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Engeström, Y. & Middleton, D. (Eds.) (1993). Cognition and communication at work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y. & Sannino, A. (2010). Studies of expansive learning: Foundations, findings and future challenges. Educational Research Review, 5, 1–24.
Engeström, Y. & Sannino, A. (2011). Discursive manifestations of contradictions in organizational change efforts: A methodological framework. Journal of Organizational Change Management, 24(3), 368–387.
Engeström, Y. & Sannino, A. (2012). Whatever happened to process theories of learning?Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 1(1), 45–56.
Engeström, Y. & Sannino, A. (2013). La volition et l’agentivité transformatrice: Perspective théorique de l’activité. Revue Internationale du CRIRES: Innover dans la tradition de Vygotsky, 1(1), 4–19.
Engeström, Y., Engeström, R. & Kärkkäinen, M. (1995). Polycontextuality and boundary crossing in expert cognition: Learning and problem solving in complex work activities. Learning and Instruction, 5, 319–336.
Engeström, Y., Engeström, R. & Vähäaho, T. (1999). When the center does not hold: The importance of knotworking. In S. Chaiklin, M. Hedegaard & U. J. Jensen (Eds.), Activity theory and social practice: Cultural-historical approaches. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.
Engeström, Y., Kerosuo, H. & Kajamaa, A. (2007). Beyond discontinuity: Expansive organizational learning remembered. Management Learning, 38, 319–336.
Engeström, Y., Lompscher, J. & Rückriem, G. (Eds.) (2005). Putting activity theory to work: Contributions from developmental work research. Berlin:Lehmanns Media.
Engeström, Y., Miettinen, R. & Punamäki, R-L. (Eds.) (1999). Perspectives on activity theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y., Nummijoki, J. & Sannino, A. (2012). Embodied germ cell at work: Building an expansive concept of physical mobility in home care. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 19(3), 287–309.
Engeström, Y., Puonti, A. & Seppänen, L. (2003). Spatial and temporal expansion of the object as a challenge for reorganizing work. In D. Nicolini, S. Gherardi & D. Yanow (Eds.), Knowing in organizations: A practice-based approach. Armonk: Sharpe.
Engeström, Y., Rantavuori, J. & Kerosuo, H. (2013). Expansive learning in a library: Actions, cycles and deviations from instructional intentions. Vocations and Learning, 6, 81–106.
Engeström, Y., Virkkunen, J., Helle, M., Pihlaja, J. & Poikela, R. (1996). Change laboratory as a tool for transforming work. Lifelong Learning in Europe, 1(2), 10–17.
Eri, T. (2013). The best way to conduct intervention research: Methodological considerations. Quality and Quantity, 47(5), 2459–2472.
Falmagne, R. J. (1995). The abstract and the concrete. In L. M. W. Martin, K. Nelson & E. Tobach (Eds.), Sociocultural psychology: Theory and practice of doing and knowing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fichtner, B. (1984). Co-ordination, co-operation and communication in the formation of theoretical concepts in instruction. In M. Hedegaard, P. Hakkarainen & Y. Engeström (Eds.), Learning and teaching on a scientific basis. Aarhus: Aarhus Universitet, Psykologisk institut (pp. 207–228).
Fichtner, B. (1985). Learning and learning activity. In E. Bol, J. P. P. Haenen & M. Wolters (Eds.), Education for cognitive development. Den Haag: SVO/SOO (pp. 47–62).
Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gagné, R. M. (1970). The conditions of learning. 2nd ed. London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Gallagher, J. M. (1978). Reflexive abstraction and education: The meaning of activity in Piaget’s theory. In J. M. Gallagher & J. A. Easley, Jr. (Eds.), Knowledge and development. Vol. 2. Piaget and education. New York: Plenum (pp. 1–20).
Gatewood, J. B. (1985). Actions speak louder than words. In J. W. D. Dougherty (Ed.), Directions in cognitive anthropology. Urbana: University of Illinois Press (pp. 199–219).
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. New York: Basic Books.
Gentner, D. & Stevens, A. L. (Eds.) (1983). Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum.
Getzels, J. W. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York: Wiley.
Giddens, A. (1982). Labour and interaction. In J. B. Thompson & D. Held (Eds.), Habermas: Critical debates. London: Macmillan (pp. 149–161).
Gladwin, H. (1985). In conclusion: Abstraction versus “how it is.” Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 16, 207–213.
Glazman, M. S. (1972). Wissenschaftliches Schöpfertum als Dialog. In G. Kröber & M. Lorf (Eds.), Wissenschaftliches Schöpfertum. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag (pp. 199–212; in German).
Goodman, N. (1978). Ways of worldmaking. Indianapolis: Hackett.
Goodman, N. (1983). Fact, fiction, and forecast. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Greeno, J. G. & Engeström, Y. (2014). Learning in activity. In R. K. Sawyer, The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Griffin, P. & Cole, M. (1984). Current activity for the future: The Zo-ped. In B. Rogoff & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), Children’s learning in the ‘zone of proximal development’. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass (pp. 45–64).
Grimshaw, A. D. (1981). Language as social resource. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Gruber, H. E. (1974). Darwin on man: A psychological study of scientific creativity. New York: Dutton.
Gruber, H. E. (1984). The emergence of a sense of purpose: A cognitive case study of young Darwin. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger (pp. 3–27).
Grünewald, G. (Ed.) (1985). Children’s campaign for nuclear disarmament. Helsinki: International Peace Bureau – Peace Union of Finland.
Grüter, B. (1979). “Dialektische Psychologie” – eine amerikanische Variante kritischer Psychologie?Forum Kritische Psychologie, 5, 157–175 (in German).
Gutiérrez, K., Baguedano-López, P. & Tejeda, C. (1999). Rethinking diversity: Hybridity and hybrid language practices in the third space. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 6(4), 286–303.
Gutiérrez, K., Rymes, B. & Larson, J. (1995). Script, counterscript, and underlife in the classroom – Brown, James versus Brown v. Board of Education. Harvard Educational Review, 65, 445–471.
Haapasaari, A., Engeström, Y. & Kerosuo, H. (in press). The emergence of learners´ transformative agency in a Change Laboratory intervention. Journal of Education and Work.
Habermas, J. (1981). Theorie des kommunikativen Handelns. Bände 1–2. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.
Habermas, J. (1984). The theory of communicative action. Vol. 1. Reason and the rationalization of society. London: Heinemann.
Haglund, B. J. A. (1983). Community diagnosis: A theoretical model for prevention in primary health care. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health Care, 1, 12–19.
Halldén, O. (1982). Elevernas tolkning av skoluppgiften. Stockholm: Pedagogiska institutionen, Stockholms Universitet (in Swedish).
Halliday, M. (1975). Learning how to mean. London: Edwin Arnold.
Hallpike, C. R. (1979). The foundations of primitive thought. New York: Oxford University Press.
Harré, R. (1970). The principles of scientific thinking. London and Basingstoke: Macmillan.
Harré, R., Clarke, D. & DeCarlo, N. (1985). Motives and mechanisms: An introduction to the psychology of action. London: Methuen.
Hasu, M. (2000). Blind men and the elephant: Implementation of a new artifact as an expansive possibility. Outlines, 2, 5–41.
Hasu, M. & Engeström, Y. (2000). Measurement in action: An activity-theoretical perspective on producer-user interaction. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 53(1), 61–90.
Haug, F. (1977). Erziehung und gesellschaftliche Produktion: Kritik des Rollenspiels. Frankfurt am Main and New York: Campus (in German).
Havelock, E. A. (1976). Origins of western literacy. Toronto: OISE.
Hegel, G. W. F. (1966). The phenomenology of mind. Translated by J. B. Baillie. 7th impression. London: George Allen & Unwin – Humanities Press.
Helenius, A. (1982). Roolileikki ja lasten suhteet (Role play and the relations of children). Jyväskylä: University of Jyväskylä. Reports from the Department of Psychology 246 (in Finnish).
Hiromatsu, K. I. (1986). “Habitus” as a key concept for understanding performance. Paper presented at the SSRC and JSPS Conference on Cognition and the Arts, Harvard (Project Zero), March 27–30, 1986.
Hirschhorn, L. (1982). The soul of a new worker. Working Papers for a New Society, 9(1), 42–47.
Hoetker, J. & Ahlbrand, W. A. (1969). The persistence of recitation. American Educational Research Journal, 6, 145–167.
Hoffman, L. (1981). Foundations of family therapy: A conceptual framework for systems change. New York: Basic Books.
Holland, D. & Reeves, J. R. (1996). Activity theory and the view from somewhere: Team perspectives on the intellectual work of programming. In B. A. Nardi (Ed.), Context and consciousness: Activity theory and human-computer interaction. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Holquist, M. (1982). Introduction. In M. M. Bakhtin, The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas Press (pp. xv–xxxiv).
Holzkamp, K. (1983). Grundlegung der Psychologie. Frankfurt am Main and New York: Campus (in German).
Holzkamp, K. (1993). Lernen: Subjektwissenschaftliche Grundlegung. Frankfurt am Main: Campus.
Hundeide, K. (1985). The tacit background of children’s judgments. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 306–322).
Ilyenkov, E. V. (1977). Dialectical logic: Essays in its history and theory. Moscow: Progress.
Ilyenkov, E. V. (1982). The dialectics of the abstract and the concrete in Marx’s “Capital.”Moscow: Progress.
Ingold, T. (2007). Lines: A brief history. Oxon: Routledge.
Jackson, R. M. (1986). Thumbs up for direct teaching of thinking skills. Educational Leadership, 43(8), 32–36.
Jensen, U. J. (1978). Über das Verhältnis zwischen Philosophie und Wissenschaft. In P. Plath & H. J. Sandkühler (Eds.), Theorie und Labor. Köln: Pahl-Rugenstein (S. 10–35; in German).
Jensen, U. J. (1981). Introduction: Preconditions for evolutionary thinking. In U. J. Jensen & R. Harré (Eds.), The philosophy of evolution. Brighton: The Harvester Press (pp. 1–22).
Joas, H. (1980). Praktische Intersubjektivität. Die Entwicklung des Werkes von G. H. Mead. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp (in German).
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Judin, E. G. (1978). Sistemnyj podchod i princip dejatel’nosti. Metodologiceskie problemy sovremennoj nauki. Moscow: Nauka (in Russian).
Jung, C. G. (1966). Two essays on analytical psychology. London and Henley: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Jungk, R. (1956). Heller als Tausend Sonnen: Das Schicksal der Atomforscher. Stuttgart: Scherz & Goverts (in German).
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. & Tversky, A. (Eds.) (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kärkkäinen, M. (1996). Comparative analysis of planning trajectories in Finnish and American teaching teams. Nordisk Pedagogik, 16, 167–190.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. & Inhelder, B. (1975). “If you want to get ahead, get a theory.” Cognition, 3, 195–212.
Kedrov, B. M. (1966–1967). On the question of the psychology of scientific creativity (on the occasion of the discovery by D. I. Mendeleev of the periodic law). Soviet Psychology, V(1), 18–37.
Kedrov, B. M. (1972). Zur Theorie der wissenschaftlichen Entdeckung. In G. Kröber & M. Lorf (Eds.) Wissenschaftliches Schöpfertum. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag, (S. 34–116; in German).
Keiler, P. (1981). Natural history and psychology: Perspectives and problems. In U. J. Jensen & R. Harré (Eds.), The philosophy of evolution. Brighton: The Harvester Press (pp. 137–154).
Keiler, P. & Schurig, V. (1978). Einige Grundlagenprobleme der Naturgeschichte des Lernens. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 3, 91–150 (in German).
Kelly, A. E., Lesh, R. A. & Baek, J. Y. (Eds.) (2008). Handbook of design research methods in education: Innovations in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics learning and teaching. New York: Routledge.
Kern, H. & Schumann, M. (1984). Das Ende der Arbeitsteilung?München: Beck (in German).
Kerosuo, H.Mäki, T. & Korpela, J. (2013). Knotworking: A novel BIM-based collaboration practice in building design projects. In Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Construction Engineering and Project Management, Orange County, California, January 9–11, 2013.
Kidder, T. (1981). The soul of a new machine. Boston: Little Brown.
Kivi, A. (1929). Seven brothers. New York: Coward – MacCann.
Klix, F. (1982). Are learning processes evolutionary invariant? An unproved assumption in psychology of learning revisited. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 190, 381–391.
Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. London: Hutchinson.
Köhler, C., Schultz-Wild, R. & Lutz, B. (1983). Flexible manufacturing systems: Manpower problems and policies. Paper presented at the World Congress on the Human Aspects of Automation, Ann Arbor, Michigan, August 8–11, 1983.
Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
Kolmogorov, A. N. 1960. Introduction in H. Lebesque, The measurement of quantities. 2nd Russian ed. Moscow: Uchpedgiz (in Russian).
Kozielecki, J. (1986). A transgressive model of man. New Ideas in Psychology 4, 89–105.
Krigman, A. (1985). Bhopal: The unanswered questions. InTech, May 1985, 9–20.
Krinsky, J. (2007). Free labor: Workfare and the contested language of neoliberalism. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Krinsky, J. (2008). Changing minds: Cognitive systems and strategic change in contention over Workfare in New York City. Social Movement Studies, 7(1), 1–29.
Krinsky, J. & Barker, C. (2009). Movement strategizing as developmental learning: Perspectives from cultural-historical activity theory. In H. Johnston (Ed.), Culture, social movements, and protest. Farnham: Ashgate.
Kruger, A. C. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Cultural learning and learning culture. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development. Malden, MA: Blackwell (pp. 369–387).
Kuchermann, R. & Wigger-Kösters, A. (1985). Die Waren laufen nicht allein zum Markt…: Die Entfaltung von Tätigkeit und Subjektitivät in der Geschichte. Köln: Pahl-Rugenstein (in German).
de Lange, T. (2011). Formal and non-formal digital practices: Institutionalizing transactional learning spaces in a media classroom. Learning, Media and Technology, 36(3), 251–275.
Langley, P. & Simon, H. (1981). The central role of learning in cognition. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 361–380).
Latour, B. & Woolgar, S. (1979). Laboratory life: The social construction of scientific facts. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Lave, J. (1985). Introduction: Situationally specific practice. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 16, 171–176.
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Leadbetter, J. (2004). The role of mediating artefacts in the work of educational psychologists during consultative conversations in schools. Educational Review, 56(2), 133–145.
Leakey, R. E. & Lewin, R. (1983). People of the lake: Mankind and its beginnings. New York: Avon Books.
Lefèvre, W. (1978). Naturtheorie und Produktionsweise. Darmstadt: Luchterhand (in German).
Lektorsky, V. A. (1984). Subject, object, cognition. Moscow: Progress.
Lenin, V. I. (1963). Philosophical notebooks. In V. I. Lenin, Collected works. Vol. 38. Moscow: Progress.
Leontjew, A. A. (1980). Tätigkeit und Kommunikation. Sowjetwissenschaft. Gesellschaftswissenschaftliche Beiträge, 33, 522–535.
Leont’ev, A. N. (1978). Activity, consciousness, and personality. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Leontiev, A. N. & Luria, A. R. (1968). The psychological ideas of L. S. Vygotskii. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Historical roots of contemporary psychology. New York: Harper & Row (pp. 338–367).
Leontyev, A. N. (1981). Problems of the development of the mind. Moscow: Progress.
Lerner, R. M. & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. (Eds.) (1981). Individuals as producers of their development: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press.
Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1980). Hand und Wort. Die Evolution von Technik, Sprache und Kunst. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp (in German).
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1961). A world on the wane. London: Hutchinson.
Levy, R. I. (1976). A conjunctive pattern in middle class informal and formal education. In T. Schwartz (Ed.), Socialization as cultural communication. Berkeley: University of California Press (pp. 177–188).
Lewontin, R. C. (1982). Organism and environment. In H. C. Plotkin (Ed.), Learning, development, and culture. New York: Wiley (pp. 151–169).
Lock, A. (Ed.) (1978). Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic Press.
Lomow, B. F. (1980). Die Kategorien Kommunikation und Tätigkeit in der Psychologie. Sowjetwissenschaft. Gesellschaftswissenschaftliche Beiträge, 33, 536–551 (in German).
Lopes, L. M. (1981). Problem solving in a human relationship: The interactional accomplishment of a “zone of proximal development” during therapy. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 3(1), 1–5.
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Malinowski, B. (1923). The problem of meaning in primitive languages. In C. K. Ogden & I. A. Richards, The meaning of meaning. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner. (pp. 296–336).
Malinowski, B. (1944). A scientific theory of culture and other essays. Chapel Hill: The University of North Carolina Press.
Marková, I. (1982). Paradigms, thought, and language. Chichester: Wiley.
Marx, K. (1909). Capital. Vol. 1. London: William Glaisher.
Marx, K. (1971). Capital. Vol. 3. Moscow: Progress.
Marx, K. (1973). Grundrisse: Foundations of the critique of political economy (rough draft). Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.
Marx, K. (1976). Theses on Feuerbach. In K. Marx & F. Engels, The German ideology. Moscow: Progress (pp. 615–617).
Marzano, R. J. & Arredondo, D. E. (1986). Restructuring schools through the teaching of thinking skills. Educational Leadership, 43(8), 20–26.
Maschewsky, W. (1977). Das Experiment in der Psychologie. Frankfurt am Main: Campus (in German).
McCarthy, T. (1984). Translator’s instroduction. In J. Habermas, The theory of communicative action. Vol. 1. Reason and the rationalization of society. London: Heinemann (pp. v–xxxvii).
McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal?Psychological Review, 92, 350–371.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Meshcheryakov, A. (1979). Awakening to life. Moscow: Progress.
Miettinen, R. (1986). The craftwork conception of science. Paper presented at the 1st International Congress of Activity Theory, West Berlin, October 3–5, 1986.
Mikhailov, F. T. (1980). The riddle of the self. Moscow: Progress.
Miller, A. I. (1984). Imagery in scientific thought: Creating 20th century physics. Boston: Birkhäuser.
Mitroff, I. I. & Kilmann, R. (1978). Methodological approaches to social science. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Mitroff, I. I. & Mason, R. O. (1981). Dialectical pragmatism: A progress report on an interdisciplinary program of research on dialectical inquiring systems. Synthese, 47, 29–42.
Morss, J. R. (1985). Old Mead in new bottles: The impersonal and the interpersonal in infant knowledge. New Ideas in Psychology, 3, 165–176.
Morss, J. R. (1986). Old Mead in new battles: A reply to Shotter. New Ideas in Psychology, 4, 85–88.
Morton, T. (2013). Hyperobjects: Philosophy and ecology after the end of the world. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Moscovici, S. (1984). Versuch über die menschliche Geschichte der Natur. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp (in German).
Mukute, M. & Lotz-Sisitka, H. (2012). Working with cultural-historical activity theory and critical realism to investigate and expand farmer learning in Southern Africa. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 19, 342–367.
Murphy, E. & Rodriguez-Manzanares, M. A. (2008). Using activity theory and its principle of contradictions to guide research in educational technology. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 24(4), 442–457.
Murphy, G. L. & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.
Nardi, B. (Ed.) (1996). Context and consciousness: Activity theory and human-computer interaction. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Nelson, K. (1979). The role of language in infant development. In M. C. Bornstein & W. Kessen (Eds.), Psychological development from infancy: Image to intention. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 307–337).
Nelson, K. (1983). The derivation of concepts and categories from event representations. In E. K. Scholnik (Ed.), New trends in conceptual representation: Challenges to Piaget’s theory?Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 129–149).
Newman, D., Griffin, P. & Cole, M. (1984). Social constraints in laboratory and classroom tasks. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (pp. 172–193).
Nickerson, R., Perkins, D. N. & Smith, E. (1985). The teaching of thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nonaka, I. & Tackeuchi, H. (1995). The knowledge-creating company: How Japanese companies create the dynamics of innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Norman, D. A. (1982). Learning and memory. San Francisco: Freeman.
Norman, D. A. (1983). Some observations on mental models. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 7–14).
Nummijoki, J. & Engeström, Y. (in preparation). Defensive and virtuous cycles of learning: A study of home care encounters.
Ogden, C. K. & Richards, I. A. (1923). The meaning of meaning. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.
Olson, D. R., Torrance, N. & Hildyard, A. (Eds.) (1985). Literacy, language, and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ong, W. J. (1977). Interfaces of the word: Studies in the evolution of consciousness and culture. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Ong, W. J. (1982). Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. London: Methuen.
Otte, M. (1980). On the question of the development of theoretical concepts. Communication & Cognition, 13(1), 63–76.
Otte, M. (1984). The work of E. G. Judin (1930–1976) on activity theory in the light of recent tendencies in epistemological thinking. In M. Hedegaard, P. Hakkarainen & Y. Engeström (Eds.), Learning and teaching on a scientific basis. Aarhus: Aarhus Universitet (pp. 43–86).
Ottomeyer, K. (1980). Marxistische Psychologie gegen Dogma und Eklektizismus. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 7, 170–207 (in German).
Parmentier, R. J. (1985). Signs’ place in medias res: Peirce’s concept of semiotic mediation. In E. Mertz & R. J. Parmentier (Eds.), Semiotic mediation: Sociocultural and psychological perspectives. Orlando, FL: Academic Press (pp. 23–48).
Peirce, C. S. (1931–1935). Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Edited by C. Hortshorne & P. Weiss. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Pharies, D. A. (1984). Charles S. Peirce and the linguistic sign. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Piaget, J. (1977). The role of action in the development of thinking. In W. F. Overton & J. M. Gallagher (Eds.), Knowledge and development. Vol. 1. Advances in research and theory. New York: Plenum (pp. 17–42).
Pihlaja, J. (2005). Learning in and for production: An activity-theoretical study of the historical development of distributed systems of generalizing. Helsinki: University of Helsinki, Department of Education.
Poddjakow, N. (1981). Die Denkentwicklung beim Vorschulkind. Berlin: Volk und Wissen (in German).
Polanyi, M. (1964). Personal knowledge: Towards a post-critical philosophy. New York: Harper & Row.
Popper, K. R. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Popper, K. R. & Eccles, J. C. (1977). The self and its brain. Berlin: Springer.
Price, D. J. de S. (1963). Little science, big science. New York: Columbia University Press.
Prigogine, I. (1984). A new model of time, a new view of physics. In J. Richardson (Ed.), Models of reality: Shaping thought and action. Mt. Airy, MD: Lomond (pp. 303–316).
Prigogine, I. (1985). Science, civilization and democracy. Keynote presentation at the 6th Parliamentary and Scientific Conference of the Council of Europe, Tokyo/Tsukuba, 3–6 June, 1985.
Prigogine, I. & Stengers, I. (1985). Order out of chaos: Man’s new dialogue with nature. London: Fontana.
Projekt Automation und Qualifikation (1980). Automationsarbeit: Empirische Untersuchungen. Teil 1. Argument-Sonderband 43. West-Berlin: Argument-Verlag (in German).
Projekt Automation und Qualifikation (1981). Automationsarbeit: Empirische Untersuchungen. Teil 2. Argument-Sonderband 55. West-Berlin: Argument-Verlag (in German).
Puonti, A. (2004). Learning to work together: Collaboration between authorities in economic-crime investigation. Vantaa: National Bureau of Investigation.
Radzikhovskii, L. A. (1984). Activity: Structure, genesis, and units of analysis. Soviet Psychology, XXII, 35–53.
Raiethel, A. (1983). Tätigkeit, Arbeit und Praxis. Frankfurt am Main and New York: Campus (in German).
Rao, H., Morrill, C. & Zald, M. N. (2000). Power plays: How social movements and collective action create new organizational forms. Research in Organizational Behavior, 22, 239–282.
Rasmussen, J. (1980). What can be learned from human error reports? In K. D. Duncan, M. M. Gruneberg & D. Wallis (Eds.), Changes in working life. Chichester: Wiley (pp. 97–114).
Rasmussen, J., Duncan, K. & Leplat, J. (Eds.) (1987). New technology and human error. Chichester: Wiley.
Ravetz, J. R. (1971). Scientific knowledge and its social problems. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Reynolds, P. C. (1981). On the evolution of human behavior. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Reynolds, P. C. (1982). The primate constructional system: The theory and description of instrumental object use in humans and chimpanzees. In M. von Cranach & R. Harré (Eds.), The analysis of action: Recent theoretical and empirical advances. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 343–386).
Richards, F. A., Armon, C. & Commons, M. L. (1984). Perspectives on the development of thought in late adolescence and adulthood: An introduction. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger (pp. xiii–xxviii).
Richardson, J. M. (1984). Global modeling in the 1980’s. In J. Richardson (Ed.), Models of reality: Shaping thought and action. Mt. Airy, MD: Lomond Books (pp. 115–129).
Riegel, K. (1973). Dialectic operations: The final period of cognitive development. Human Development, 16, 346–370.
Riegel, K. F. (1979). Foundations of dialectical psychology. New York: Academic Press.
Rogoff, B. & Lave, J. (Eds.) (1984). Everyday cognition: Development in social context. Cambrdige, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rogoff, B. & Wertsch, J. V. (Eds.) (1984). Children’s learning in the “zone of proximal development.”San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Roset, I. (1984). The psychology of fantasy. Moscow: Progress.
Rouse, W. B. & Morris, N. M. (1985). On looking into the black box: Prospects and limits in the search for mental models. Center for Man-Machine Systems Research. Georgia Institute of Technology. Report no. 85–2.
Ruben, P. (1978). Dialektik und Arbeit der Philosophie. Köln: Pahl-Rugenstein (in German).
Ruben, P. (1981). From moralization to class society or from class society to moralization: Philosophical comments on Klaus Eder’s hypothesis. In U. J. Jensen & R. Harré (Eds.), The philosophy of evolution. Brighton: The Harvester Press (pp. 120–136).
Saarelma, O. (1993). Descriptions of subjective networks as a mediator of developmental dialogue. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 15, 102–112.
Sannino, A. (2011). Activity theory as an activist and interventionist theory. Theory and Psychology, 21(5), 571–597.
Schmandt-Basserat, D. (1978). The earliest precursor of writing. Scientific American, 238(6), 50–59.
Schön, D. A. (1983). The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action. London: Temple Smith.
Schurig, V. (1976). Die Entstehung des Bewusstseins. Frankfurt am Main and New York: Campus.
Scribner, S. (1985). Vygotsky’s uses of history. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 119–145).
Scribner, S. & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Seidel, R. (1976). Denken – psychologische Analyse der Entstehung und Lösung von Problemen. Frankfurt am Main/New York: Campus (in German).
Selz, O. (1924). Gesetze der produktiven und reproduktiven Geistestätigkeit. Bonn: Cohen (in German).
Sharp, D. W., Cole, M. & Lave, C. (1979). Education and cognitive development: The evidence from experimental research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44(1–2, Serial No. 178).
Shotter, J. (1982). Consciousness, self-consciousness, inner games, and alternative realities. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Aspects of consciousness. Vol. 3. Awareness and self-awareness. London: Academic Press (pp. 27–62).
Shotter, J. (1986). Realism and relativism, rules and intentionality, theories and accounts: A response to Morss. New Ideas in Psychology, 4, 71–84.
Sigel, I. E. (1983). Is the concept of the concept still elusive or what do we know about concept development? In E. K. Scholnick (Ed.), New trends in conceptual representation: Challenges to Piaget’s theory?Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 239–273).
Simon, H. A. (1983). Reason in human affairs. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Sluzki, C. E. & Ransom, D. C. (Eds.) (1976). Double bind: The foundation of the communication approach to the family. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Snyder, B. R. (1971). The hidden curriculum. New York: Knopf.
Soule, S. A. (2012). Social movements and markets, industries and firms. Organization Studies, 33(12), 1715–1733.
Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Svechnikov, G. A. (1971). Causality and the relation of states in physics. Moscow: Progress.
Toikka, K. (1986). Development of work in FMS – case study on new manpower strategy. Paper presented in the IFAC Workshop “Skill Based Automated Manufacturing,” Karlsruhe, FRG, September 3–5, 1986.
Toikka, K., Engeström, Y. & Norros, L. (1985). Entwickelnde Arbeitsforschung: Theoretische and methdologische Elemente (Developmental work research: Theoretical and methodological elements). Forum Kritische Psychologie, 15, 5–41 (in German).
Toikka, K., Hyötyläinen, R. & Norros, L. (1986). Development of work in flexible manufacturing. Nordisk Pedagogik, 6(1), 16–24.
Toiviainen, H. (2007). Inter-organizational learning across levels: An object-oriented approach. Journal of Workplace Learning, 19(6), 343–358.
Tolman, C. (1981). The metaphysic of relations in Klaus Riegel’s “dialectics” of human development. Human Development, 24, 33–51.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Toulmin, S. (1982). The return to cosmology: Postmodern science and the theology of nature. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Trân Duc Thao (1984). Investigations into the origin of language and consciousness. Dordrecht: Reidel.
Trevarthen, C. & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic Press (pp. 183–230).
Tsuckerman, G. A. (2011). Developmental education: A genetic modeling experiment. Journal ofRussian and East European Psychology, 49(6), 45–63.
Tuomi-Gröhn, T. & Engeström, Y. (Eds.) (2003). Between school and work: New perspectives on transfer and boundary crossing. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Twain, M. (1950). The adventures of Huckleberry Finn. London: The Cresset Press.
Tweney, R. D., Doherty, M. E. & Mynatt, C. R. (1981). Epilogue. In R. D. Tweney, M. E. Doherty & C. R. Mynatt (Eds.), On scientific thinking. New York: Columbia University Press (pp. 399–417).
Ushakova, T. N. (1977). Causes of children’s word invention (A psychophysiological model of the genesis of the syntactically structured verbal utterance). In M. Cole (Ed.), Soviet developmental psychology: An anthology. White Plains, NY: M. E. Sharpe (pp. 516–537).
van der Veer, R. & Valsiner, J. (1991). Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. Oxford: Blackwell.
Virkkunen, J. (2006). Dilemmas in building shared transformative agency. Activités, 3(1), 44–66.
Virkkunen, J. (2009). Two theories of organizational knowledge creation. In A. Sannino, H. Daniels & K. D. Gutiérrez (Eds.), Learning and expanding with activity theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 144–159).
Virkkunen, J. & Newnham, D. S. (2013). The Change Laboratory: A tool for collaborative development of work and education. Rotterdam: Sense.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1971). The psychology of art. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The instrumental method in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, NY: Sharpe (pp. 134–143).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Lectures on psychology. In The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol. 1. Problems of general psychology. New York: Plenum (pp. 289–358).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997a). The historical meaning of the crisis in psychology: A methodological investigation. In The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol. 3. Problems of the theory and history of psychology. New York: Plenum (pp. 223–343).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997b). The history of the development of higher mental functions. In The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol. 4. The history of the development of higher mental functions. New York: Plenum (pp. 1–251).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997c). The instrumental method in psychology. In The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol. 3. Problems of the theory and history of psychology. New York: Plenum (pp. 85–89).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1999). Tool and sign in the development of the child. In The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol. 6. Scientific legacy. New York: Kluwer/Plenum (pp. 1–68).
Wales, C. E., Nardi, A. H. & Stager, R. A. (1986). Decision making: New paradigm for education. Educational Leadership, 43(8), 37–41.
Wartofsky, M. (1968). Conceptual foundations of scientific thought: An introduction to the philosophy of science. New York: MacMillan.
Wartofsky, M. (1979). Models: Representation and scientific understanding. Dordrecht: Reidel.
Wartofsky, M. (1983). From genetic epistemology to historical epistemology: Kant, Marx, and Piaget. In L. S. Liben (Ed.), Piaget and the foundations of knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 1–18).
Weinberg, A. M. (1967). Reflections of big science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Werner, H. (1961). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: Science Editions.
Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper & Brothers.
Wertsch, J. V. (Ed.) (1985a). Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wertsch, J. V. (1985b). The semiotic mediation of mental life: L. S. Vygotsky and M. M. Bakhtin. In E. Mertz & R. J. Parmentier (Eds.), Semiotic mediation: Sociocultural and psychological perspectives. Orlando: Academic Press (pp. 49–72).
Wertsch, J. V. (1985c). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilde, L. (1989). Marx and contradiction. Aldershot: Avebury.
Wilhelmer, B. (1979). Lernen als Handlung. Köln: Pahl-Rugenstein (in German).
Wood, D. J. (1980). Teaching the young child: Some relationships between social interaction, language, and thought. In D. R. Olson (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought. New York: Norton (pp. 280–298).
Wood, D., Bruner, J. S. & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.
Wood, S. (Ed.) (1982). The degradation of work?London: Hutchinson.
Wood, S. (1987). The deskilling debate, new technology and work organization. Acta Sociologica, 30, 3–24.
Wygotski, L. S. (1977). Denken und Sprechen. Frankfurt am Main: Fischer (in German).
Zilsel, E. (1976). Die sozialen Ursprünge der neuzeitlichen Wissenschaft. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp (in German).
Zinchenko, P. I. (1983–84). The problem of involuntary memory. Soviet Psychology, XXII, 55–111.
Zinchenko, V. P. (1985). Vygotsky’s ideas about units for the analysis of mind. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 94–118).
Zivin, G. (Ed.) (1979). The development of self-regulation through private speech. New York: Wiley.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.