Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-07T14:33:37.984Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Sex, Gender, and Intelligence

Does XX = XY for Intelligence?

from Section 3 - Cognitive and Social Factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 July 2020

Fanny M. Cheung
Affiliation:
The Chinese University of Hong Kong
Diane F. Halpern
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College, California
Get access

Summary

We conclude that there are no overall (average) differences between women and men in general intelligence, but there are some large and persistent differences on cognitive abilities that on average favor males (e.g. mathematics, mental rotation, mechanical) or favor females (verbal ability, most tests of memory). There are more males in the low end of the intelligence distribution, at least in part, for sex-related genetic reasons. There is no genetic evidence for more males in the high end of the intelligence distribution. Paradoxically, societies with greater gender equality do not show reduced differences on many cognitive measures. Our conclusions are about group differences. Thus, these mean differences have no clinical or social significance at the individual level.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Suggested Readings

To give readers a broad coverage of the field we suggest reading articles written from various perspectives. We hope that the contradictions in these readings will encourage readers to think critically about this controversial topic.

Diane F. Halpern is Professor of Psychology Emerita from Claremont McKenna College and Dean of Social Sciences Emerita from Minerva Schools at KGI. She has published many books and hundreds of journal articles on sex differences in cognitive abilities and critical thinking, and is the author of the Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment (HCTA). She is past president of the American Psychological Association, the Western Psychological Association, and the Society for the Teaching of Psychology. She was named the Federation of Associations in Behavioral & Brain Sciences (FABBS) Honored Scientist (IHO) “Who made important and lasting contributions to the science of mind, brain, and behavior.” www.fabbs.org/fabbs-foundation/honoring-our-scientists/honor-diane-f-ha/

Halpern was born in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. In order to get a better understanding of psychology from numerous cultures, she taught courses (most of them a semester long) in Canada, Mexico, Turkey, Russia, China, and Portugal, among other places. Her parents immigrated to the United States to escape the dangerous growth of anti-Semitism in Europe. Her father was from Romania and her mother was from Poland. Her first language was Yiddish, which developed as a fusion of German, Hebrew, Aramaic, and Slavic languages. She has a racially mixed family.

Carmen Flores-Mendoza is a psychologist at Federal University of Minas Gerais, Brazil. She works on individual differences in intelligence and personality, especially in the Latin American context. Flores-Mendoza was born in Lima, Peru. In the 1980s, her family moved to Brazil to escape the armed conflict (internal war) between the government of Peru and the Communist Party of Peru (a terrorist group known as Shining Path). The armed conflict turned Peru into a country where it was not safe to live. Now she has two nationalities: Peruvian and Brazilian.  She attended University in the state of São Paulo, Brazil, and received her PhD from University of São Paulo. Her paternal grandmother was Chinese, and her paternal grandfather was indigenous Peruvian. Her maternal grandmother was “mulatto” (Black + White), and her maternal grandfather was a Spaniard (from Spain). So, Flores-Mendoza represents a mix of races.

Heiner Rindermann is a psychologist at TU Chemnitz, Germany, and works on education, ability development, intelligence, student achievement, economy and culture, and their interplay at the level of individuals and societies. Rindermann was born in Cologne, Germany, and grew up in Baden-Württemberg. He received his PhD from Heidelberg University. From 2008 to 2010, he was Professor of Evaluation and Methodology of Developmental Psychology at the University of Graz in Austria. He is a member of ISIR, APS, AEA, and Heterodox Academy. Science is for him an intellectual-philosophical task. He likes to walk, camp, and swim in the wilderness. He also likes to take photos (nature, architecture, art, people).

Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 218261. doi:10.1037/a0014412Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (2012). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, W., Carothers, A., & Deary, I. J. (2008). Sex differences in variability in general intelligence: A new look at the old question. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 518531. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00096.xGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2004). Sex differences on the progressive matrices: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 32, 481498. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.008Google Scholar
Nyborg, H. (2003). Sex differences in g. In Nyborg, H. (Ed.), The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (pp. 187222). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Wai, J., Cacchio, M., Putallaz, M., & Makel, M.C. (2010). Sex differences in the right tail of cognitive abilities: A 30-year examination. Intelligence, 38, 412423. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Allingham, R. R. (2008). Assessment of visual status of the Aeta, a hunter-gatherer population of the Philippines. Transactions of the American Ophthalmological Society, 106, 240251.Google Scholar
Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., Peterson, R. L., Willcutt, E. G., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2017). Explaining the sex difference in dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 58, 719727. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12691Google Scholar
Bassi, M., Busso, M., & Muñoz, J. S. (2013). Is the glass half empty or half full? School enrollment, graduation, and dropout rates in Latin America. www.iadb.org/en/research-and-data/publication-details-working-papers?pub_id=IDB-WP-462Google Scholar
Becker, D., & Rindermann, H. (2017). Cognitive sex differences: Evolution and history. Mankind Quarterly, 58, 8392.Google Scholar
Blinkhorn, S. (2005). A gender bender. Nature, 438, 3132. doi:10.1038/438031aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braga, L. S., Flores-Mendoza, C., Barroso, S. M., Saldanha, R. S., Santos, M. T., Akama, C. T., & Reis, M. C. (2014). Diferenças de sexo em uma habilidade cognitiva específica e na produção científica [Sex Differences in a cognitive ability measure and in scientific production]. Psico-USF, 19(3), 477487. doi:10.1590/1413-82712014019003010Google Scholar
Cashdan, E., & Gaulin, S. J. (2016). Why go there? Evolution of mobility and spatial cognition in women and men. Human Nature, 27, 115. doi:10.1007/s12110–015-9253-4Google Scholar
Cavaco, S., Goncalves, A., Pinto, C., Almeida, E., Gomes, F., Moreira, I., … Teixeira-Pinto, A. (2015). Auditory verbal learning test in a large nonclinical Portuguese population. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 22, 321331. doi:10.1080/23279095.2014.927767Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 218261. doi:10.1037/a0014412Google Scholar
Chuck, E. (2018). James Damore, Google engineer fired for writing manifesto on women’s “neuroticism,” sues company. www.nbcnews.com/news/us-news/google-engineer-fired-writing-manifesto-women-s-neuroticism-sues-company-n835836.Google Scholar
Citeli, M. T. (2001). Fazendo diferenças: Teorias sobre gênero, corpo e comportamento. Estudos Feministas, 9(1), 131145. doi:10.1590/S0104–026X2001000100007Google Scholar
Cooper, C. (2015). Intelligence and human abilities. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Coutrot, A., Silva, R., Manley, E., de Cothi, W., Sami, S., Bohbot, V. D., … Spiers, H. J. (2018). Global determinants of navigation ability. Current Biology, 28(17), 28612866. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.009Google Scholar
Crow, J. F. (2002). Unequal by nature: A geneticist’s perspective on human differences. Dædalus, 131, 8188.Google Scholar
Damore, J. (2017). Google’s ideological echo chamber. firedfortruth.com/2017/08/08/first-blog-postGoogle Scholar
Daseking, M., Petermann, F., & Waldmann, H.-C. (2017). Sex differences in cognitive abilities: Analyses for the German WAIS-IV. Personality and Individual Differences, 114, 145150. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.003Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2008 [1982]). The extended phenotype: The long reach of the gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eliez, S., Blasey, C. M., Freund, L. S., Hastie, T., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Brain anatomy, gender, and IQ in children and adolescents with Fragile X syndrome. Brain, 124, 16101618. doi:10.1093/brain/124.8.1610Google Scholar
Ellis, L., Hershberger, S., Field, E., Wersinger, S., Pellis, S., Geary, D., … Karadi, K. (2008). Sex differences: Summarizing more than a century of scientific research. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2000). Sexing the body: Gender politics and the construction of sexuality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Flore, P. C., & Wicherts, J. M. (2015). Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 53, 2544. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002Google Scholar
Flores-Mendoza, C., Ardila, R., Rosas, R., Lucio, M. E., Gallegos, M., & Colareto, N. R. (2018). Intelligence measurement and school performance in Latin America. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-89975-6Google Scholar
Flores-Mendoza, C., Widaman, K. F., Rindermann, H., Primi, R., Mansur-Alves, M., & Couto Pena, C. (2013). Cognitive sex differences in reasoning tasks: Evidence from Brazilian samples. Intelligence, 41, 7084. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2012.11.002Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2017). Male and female balance sheet. Mankind Quarterly, 58, 4368.Google Scholar
Forstmeier, W. (2011). Women have relatively larger brains than men: A comment on the misuse of the general linear model in the study of sexual dimorphism. Anatomical Record, 294, 18561863. doi:10.1002/ar.21423Google Scholar
Geiser, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2008). A note on sex differences in mental rotation in different age groups. Intelligence, 36, 556563. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.12.003Google Scholar
Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Gender, culture, and math. Science, 320, 11641165. doi:10.1126/science.1154094Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (2012). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 151. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.xGoogle Scholar
Holden, M. P., Duff-Canning, S. J., & Hampson, E. (2015). Sex differences in the weighting of metric and categorical information in spatial location memory. Psychological Research, 79, 118. doi:10.1007/s00426–013-0539-zGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. (2011). Human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Iliescu, D., Ilie, A., Ispas, D., Dobrean, A., & Clinciu, A. I. (2016). Sex differences in intelligence: A multi-measure approach using nationally representative samples from Romania. Intelligence, 58, 5461. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2016.06.007Google Scholar
Irwing, P., & Lynn, R. (2005). Sex differences in means and variability on the Progressive Matrices in university students: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 505524. doi:10.1348/000712605X53542Google Scholar
Jenson, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Johnson, W., Carothers, A., & Deary, I. J. (2008). Sex differences in variability in general intelligence: A new look at the old question. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 518531. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00096.xGoogle Scholar
Jussim, L. (2012). Social perception and social reality: Why accuracy dominates bias and self-fulfilling prophecy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195366600.001.0001Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B. (2007). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory? Intelligence, 35, 211223. Doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.009Google Scholar
Keiser, H. N., Sackett, P. R., Kuncel, N. R., & Brothen, T. (2016). Why women perform better in college than admissions scores would predict: Exploring the role of conscientiousness and course-taking patterns. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 569581. doi:10.1037/apl0000069Google Scholar
Keith, T. Z., Reynolds, M. R., Patel, P. G., & Ridley, K. P. (2008). Sex differences in latent cognitive abilities ages 6 to 59: Evidence from the Woodcock-Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 36, 502525. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.11.001Google Scholar
Lai, D., Tseng, Y., & Guo, H. (2011). Gender and geographic differences in developmental delays among young children: Analysis of the data from the national registry in Taiwan. Research In Developmental Disabilities, 32, 6369. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.08.012Google Scholar
Lai, D.-C., Tseng, Y.-C., Hou, Y.-M., & Guo, H. (2012). Gender and geographic differences in the prevalence of intellectual disability in children: Analysis of data from the national disability registry of TaiwanResearch In Developmental Disabilities33, 23012307. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.07.001Google Scholar
Lippa, R. A., Collaer, M. L., & Peters, M. (2010). Sex differences in mental rotation and line angle judgments are positively associated with gender equality and economic development across 53 nations. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 990997. doi:10.1007/s10508–008-9460-8Google Scholar
Loomes, R., Hull, L., & Mandy, W. P. L. (2017). What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56, 466474. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013Google Scholar
Lovén, J., Rehnman, J., Wiens, S., Lindholm, T., Peira, N., & Herlitz, A. (2012). Who are you looking at? The influence of face gender on visual attention and memory for own- and other-race faces. Memory, 20, 321331. doi:10.1080/09658211.2012.658064Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (1994). Sex differences in brain size and intelligence: A paradox resolved. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 257271. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90030-2Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (2017). Sex differences in intelligence: The developmental theory. Mankind Quarterly, 58, 942.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2004). Sex differences on the progressive matrices: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 32, 481498. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.008Google Scholar
Makel, M. C., Wai, J., Peairs, K. F., & Putallaz, M. (2016). Sex differences in the right tail of cognitive abilities: An update and cross cultural extension. Intelligence, 59, 815.doi:10.1016/j.intell.2016.09.003Google Scholar
Meisenberg, G. (2016). Gender differences in school achievement across cultures: An analysis of results from PISA 2000-2012. Mankind Quarterly, 57, 227251.Google Scholar
Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). Can spatial training improve long-term outcomes for gifted STEM undergraduates? Learning and Individual Differences26 141152. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.03.012Google Scholar
Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 3745. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011Google Scholar
Muller, C. B., Ride, S. M., Fouke, J., Whitney, T., Denton, D. D., Cantor, N., … Robinson, S. (2005). Gender differences and performance in science. Science, 307, 1043. doi:10.1126/science.307.5712.1043bGoogle Scholar
Murray, C. (2003). Human accomplishment: The pursuit of excellence in the arts and sciences, 800 B.C. to 1950. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Nyborg, H. (2003). Sex differences in g. In Nyborg, H. (Ed.), The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen. (pp. 187222). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Nyborg, H. (2015). Sex differences across different racial ability levels. Intelligence, 52, 4462. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.005Google Scholar
O’Kelly, C. G. (1980). Women and men in society. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Owen, L. (2005). Distorting the past: Gender and the division of labor in the European Upper Paleolithic. Tubingen: Kerns Verlag.Google Scholar
Parguliski, J. R., & Reynolds, M. R. (2017). Sex differences in achievement: Distributions matter. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 272278. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.16Google Scholar
Parsons, T. D., Larson, P., Kratz, K., Thiebaux, M., Bluestein, B., Buckwalter, J. G., & Rizzo, A. A. (2004). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment. Neuropsychologia, 42, 555562. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.08.014Google Scholar
Richardson, J. T. E. (1994). Gender differences in mental rotation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 435448. doi:10.2466/pms.1994.78.2.435Google Scholar
Rindermann, H. (2009). Emotionale-Kompetenz-Fragebogen (EKF). [Emotional-Competence-Questionnaire (EKF).] Göttingen: Hogrefe. doi:10.1017/9781107279339Google Scholar
Rindermann, H. (2018). Cognitive capitalism: Human capital and the wellbeing of nations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ritchie, S. (2015). Intelligence. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Ritchie, S. J., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 29, 13581369. doi:10.1177/0956797618774253Google Scholar
Romanoff, S. (1983). Women as hunters among the Matses of the Peruvian Amazon. Human Ecology, 11, 339343. doi:10.1007/BF00891379Google Scholar
Rustoyburu, C. (2012). Infancia, hormonas y género. Un análisis histórico de los discursos de la biotipología en Argentina en los años 1930. Sexualidad, Salud y Sociedad – Revista Latinoamericana, 11, 936.Google Scholar
Salzano, F. M., & Sans, M. (2014). Interethnic admixture and the evolution of Latin American populations. Genetics and Molecular Biology, 31, 1, 151170. doi:10.1590/S1415–47572014000200003Google Scholar
Saß, S., Kampa, N., & Köller, O. (2017). The interplay of g and mathematical abilities in large-scale assessments across grades. Intelligence, 63, 3344. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2017.05.001Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype → environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424435.Google Scholar
Singh, K. S. (2001). Gender roles in history: Women as hunters. Gender, Technology, and Development, 5, 113124. doi:10.1177/097185240100500105Google Scholar
Skarbrevik, K. J. (2010). Gender differences among students found eligible for special education. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 17, 97107, doi:10.1080/08856250210129038aGoogle Scholar
Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2015). Sex differences in academic achievement are not related to political, economic, or social equality. Intelligence, 48, 137151. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.006Google Scholar
Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2018). The gender-equality paradox in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics education. Psychological Science, 29, 581593. doi:10.1177/0956797617741719Google Scholar
Summers, L. H. (2005). Remarks at NBER conference on diversifying the science and engineering workforce. www.harvard.edu/president/speeches/summers_2005/nber.phpGoogle Scholar
Tang, H., Quertermous, T., Rodriguez, B., Kardia, S. L. R., Zhu, X., Brown, A., … Risch, N. J. (2005). Genetic structure, self-identified race/ethnicity, and confounding in case-control association studies. American Journal of Human Genetics, 76(2), 268275. doi:10.1086/427888Google Scholar
Thompson, A. E., & Voyer, D. (2014). Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 11641195. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.875889Google Scholar
Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 352402. doi:10.1037/a0028446Google Scholar
Valtonen, R., Ahonen, T., Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2004). Co-occurrence of developmental delays in a screening study of 4-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 46(7), 436443. doi:10.1017/S0012162204000726Google Scholar
Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 11741204. doi:10.1037/a0036620Google Scholar
Voyer, D., Voyer, S. D., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2017). Sex differences in visual-spatial working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 307334. doi:10.3758/s13423–016-1085-7Google Scholar
Wai, J., Cacchio, M., Putallaz, M., & Makel, M.C. (2010). Sex differences in the right tail of cognitive abilities: A 30 year examination. Intelligence, 38, 412423. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.006Google Scholar
Wai, J., & Putallaz, M. (2011). The Flynn effect puzzle: A 30-year examination from the right tail of the ability distribution provides some missing pieces. Intelligence, 39, 443455. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.07.006Google Scholar
Wakabayashi, D., & Bowles, N. (2018). Google memo author sues, claiming bias against White conservative men. New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2018/01/08/technology/google-memo-discrimination-lawsuit.htmlGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×