Elsevier

The Spine Journal

Volume 17, Issue 4, April 2017, Pages 505-510
The Spine Journal

Clinical Study
Waddell non-organic signs: new evidence suggests somatic amplification among outpatient chronic pain patients

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spinee.2016.10.018Get rights and content

Abstract

Background Context

Waddell et al. identified a set of eight non-organic signs in 1980. There has been controversy about their meaning, particularly with respect to their use as validity indicators.

Purpose

The current study examined the Waddell signs in relation to measures of somatic amplification or over-reporting in a sample of outpatient chronic pain patients. We examined the degree to which these signs were associated with measures of over-reporting.

Study Design/Setting

This study examined scores on the Waddell signs in relation to over-reporting indicators in an outpatient chronic pain sample.

Patient Sample

We examined 230 chronic pain patients treated at a multidisciplinary pain clinic. The majority of these patients presented with primary back or spinal injuries.

Outcome Measures

The outcome measures used in the study were Waddell signs, Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire, Pain Disability Index, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 Restructured Form.

Methods

We examined Waddell signs using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) and analysis of variance (ANOVA), receiver operating characteristic analysis, classification accuracy, and relative risk ratios.

Results

Multivariate analysis of variance and ANOVA showed a significant association between Waddell signs and somatic amplification. Classification analyses showed increased odds of somatic amplification at a Waddell score of 2 or 3.

Conclusions

Our results found significant evidence of an association between Waddell signs and somatic over-reporting. Elevated scores on the Waddell signs (particularly scores higher than 2 and 3) were associated with increased odds of exhibiting somatic over-reporting.

Introduction

In 1980, Waddell et al. [1] developed a systematic collection of eight physical signs (widely referred to as Waddell signs) thought to measure non-organic subjective pain complaints centered around the lower back and extremities. These signs, reflective of pain complaints, did not have an organic etiology and were originally proposed to objectively predictwhether a patient would be a successful back surgery candidate. Waddell et al. proposed that the signs might reduce lengthy and costly referrals for psychological testing, which is sometimes used to identify poor surgical candidacy [1]. Fishbain et al. [2] reviewed 61 studies involving the Waddell signs and summarized the literature at the time. The authors found consistent evidence that the non-organic signs demonstrated acceptable inter-rater reliability, were associated with poorer surgical treatment outcomes, and predicted prolonged non-return to work.

The utility of Waddell signs as indicators of symptom feigning has been debated since their introduction, and some have pointed out that the signs are abused by physicians to discredit the validity of patients' complaints [3]. Fishbain et al. [4] concluded that Waddell signs were not representative of secondary gain and malingering because they were not consistently associated with medicolegal or workers' compensation status and improved with treatment. Fishbain et al. found mixed results as to whether Waddell scores were related to physicians' perception of dishonesty. Fishbain et al. also discussed three studies that found no association between Waddell signs and Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI)/Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) validity scales, thus supporting their claim that the signs were not representative of symptom over-reporting [1], [5], [6]. However, the three studies that examined the Waddell and MMPI/MMPI-2 validity scales limited their investigation to the three original validity indicators on the test: L, F, and K. Two of these scales (L and K) are measures of under-reporting and would therefore not be expected to show any significant association with Waddell signs. The F scale of the MMPI and MMPI-2 measures feigned psychopathology and, as such, would be unlikely to capture over-reporting of somatic and pain symptoms [7], [8].

The current study examined the Waddell signs in a sample of chronic pain patients treated at an outpatient multidisciplinary clinic. The majority of these patients had external incentives in the form of disability involvement and seeking narcotic pain medication, which is another secondary gain issue common in this type of setting. Scores on the Waddell signs were compared with various self-report indicators of over-reporting (both somatic and psychological symptoms), as well as systematic assessment of psychological, somatic, and pain variables. We hypothesized that individuals receiving higher scores on the Waddell signs would show evidence of somatic and pain over-reporting. We examined Waddell scores in relation to the latest version of the MMPI, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 Restructured Form (MMPI-2-RF) [9], which includes two validity scales specifically designed to assess non-credible somatic complaints, the Infrequent Somatic Responses (Fs) and Symptom Validity (FBS-r) scales, as well as two brief self-report measures of somatic and pain perception, the Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire (MSPQ) [10] and the Pain Disability Index (PDI) [11], both of which have been shown to be effective in capturing amplification of somatic symptoms and pain [12], [13], [14].

Section snippets

Participants

Evidence & Methods

Context

Waddell's signs have been used as a screening utility for the detection of non-organic pain generation and amplification since they were first described more than 30 years ago. The authors sought to evaluate the correlation between documented Waddell's clinical signs and somatic amplification or over-reporting in a chronic pain population.

Contribution

This study included 230 patients. In this analysis, Waddell's signs were associated with increased likelihood of somatic

Group comparisons

We examined evidence of symptom over-reporting as a function of performance on the Waddell signs. We classified patients into three groups: Waddell=0, Waddell=1–2, and Waddell>2. Next, we analyzed the differences between the three groups, using a one-way multivariate analysis of variance with respect to indicators of symptom amplification. We conducted this analysis with the expectation that patients in the higher Waddell scores group would score higher on various indicators of symptom

Discussion

Previous reviews by Fishbain et al. [2], [4] have suggested that Waddell signs are not reflective of secondary gain or malingering, in part because of their lack of association with MMPI/MMPI-2 validity scales. However, as noted earlier, it is not surprising that Waddell signs showed no association with the MMPI markers of defensive responding (L and K scales) and exaggerated psychopathology (F scale). Simply, these studies were looking predominately at exaggerated emotional and psychological

Conclusions

A single finding of over-reporting does not equate to a classification of malingering as current standards for malingering assessment require multiple findings. Classification of malingering requires consideration of a number of factors in addition to evidence of response bias and symptom amplification, such as the presence of secondary gain, motivation, and evaluation context. Thus, we do not propose that Waddell signs provide conclusive evidence of malingering in isolation; rather, our

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge Martin Sellbom, Jonathan Gore, and Adam Crighton for their assistance with some of the statistical analyses in this project.

References (20)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (8)

  • Cues for feigning screening in whiplash associated disorders

    2021, Legal Medicine
    Citation Excerpt :

    On the other hand, Boskovic et al. [8] evaluated the effectiveness of the Verifiability Approach [36] to assess the symptom exageration, obtaining significant results but finding the same problem as the previous authors: the tools used are not entirely effective due to the fact that they are designed to evaluate testimonies, or subjects with very specific characteristics, which is not usually common in the medical-legal context. Other research has attempted to validate exclusively behavioral indicators, known as non-organic signs, that can be used to identify malingering of physical symptoms such as pain [54], but the studies present various theoretical and empirical limitations that compromise the usefulness of their results. Hence, to date, it has not been possible to associate specific behavioral or verbal indicators with feigning or malingering [23].

  • The negative Hawthorne effect: Explaining pain overexpression

    2019, Joint Bone Spine
    Citation Excerpt :

    Patients may exaggerate their complaints to obtain compensation for a work-related injury or to perpetuate a situation they feel is advantageous to them, for instance if they are seeking financial reparation or disability status, a sick leave extension, or the granting of social benefits that depend on being scored above a severity cutoff. This type of negative Hawthorne effect is known as malingering, which is intermediate between simulation and accident neurosis [22]. Patients who are frustrated about not achieving the expected level of relief may exaggerate their complaints to emphasize the inadequacy of their management, even when they have received the best care possible.

  • The negative Hawthorne effect: Explaining pain overexpression

    2019, Revue du Rhumatisme (Edition Francaise)
  • You'd Better Believe It: The Conceptual and Practical Challenges of Assessing Malingering in Patients With Chronic Pain

    2019, Journal of Pain
    Citation Excerpt :

    Although pain behaviors may be genuine responses to pain, they may also be the product of other reinforcement contingencies and can be deliberately exaggerated and/or deceptive. Consequently, studies have investigated the possibility that such behaviors can be used to identify over-reporting of physical symptoms52 to support a diagnosis of malingering.16,134 In an effort to standardize the measurement of pain behaviors in patients with back pain, Waddell et al122 identified 8 common signs or behaviors that they considered not attributable to organic causes or structural pathology.

  • Using the MMPI-3 in forensic psychological assessments

    2022, Clinical Forensic Psychology: Introductory Perspectives on Offending
  • The Role of Waddell Signs in Low Back Pain

    2021, Anesthesia and Analgesia
View all citing articles on Scopus

FDA device/drug status: Not applicable.

Author disclosures: DBW: Nothing to disclose. PAA: Nothing to disclose. KJB: Nothing to disclose. RLU: Nothing to disclose.

View full text