Elsevier

Biological Psychology

Volume 77, Issue 3, March 2008, Pages 343-352
Biological Psychology

The impact of social context on mimicry

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.008Get rights and content

Abstract

Facial mimicry, the tendency to imitate other's facial expressions, has frequently been described as a reflex-like mechanism that function independent of the relationship between expresser and observer. However, there is also evidence suggesting that it is a social cue regulating social interactions and that consequently mimicry varies as a function of social context and the type of emotion expression shown. Two studies were conducted to the assess impact of social group membership and type of expression on facial mimicry. Results suggest that the level of facial mimicry varies as a function of group membership. Moreover, mimicry levels were influenced by the kind of emotion displayed by the expresser. Although participants mimicked happiness displays regardless of the expresser's group membership, negative emotions were either not mimicked or only when shown by an ingroup member.

Section snippets

Impact of social context on mimicry

Everyone can recall wincing at the sight of someone's expression of pain after being hurt, or smiling when a friend, overwhelmed with joy, tells us something great happened to him. These behaviors, mimicry, are an essential part of interactions in everyday life.

Mimicry is usually defined as the tendency to imitate facially, vocally or posturally people with whom we are interacting (e.g., Hess et al., 1999). There is ample evidence for the occurrence of mimicry in both children (e.g., Chisholm

Study 1

Study 1 was conducted to assess whether social group membership defined through shared fundamental attitudes influences mimicry. For this, we adapted the study by McHugo et al. (1991) described above, who found that mimicry and emotional contagion effects associated with happiness displays were influenced by political attitudes. Yet, one possible problem with this study is that the two politicians differed in their general appeal. Reagan was a much more charismatic person and rather expressive.

Participants

A total of 54 students (19 men, 25 women, and 10 gender unknown) from the University of Quebec at Montreal with a mean age of 25 years participated individually. On average, participants rated their political attitudes as 4.2 out of 6 on the Quebec Nationalism Scale (Guimond and Dubé-Simard, 1983), which strongly reflects Bouchard's political position.

Facial stimuli

Twelve short video sequences of expressions displayed by Jean Charest and Lucien Bouchard (three smiles and three frowns each), lasting 13.2 s on

Manipulation check

There were no differences in personal attitude ratings for the two politicians on the dominance (Bouchard: M = 109.57, S.D. = 80.42; Charest: M = 112.18, S.D. = 82.34) t(53) = .190, p = .850), the affiliation (Bouchard: M = 18.11, S.D. = 109.86; Charest: M = 32.02, S.D. = 75.95), t(53) = .703, p = .485), or the pleasantness (Bouchard: M = 82.19, S.D. = 40.16; Charest: M = 76.31, S.D. = 34.80, t(53) = 1.019, p = .313) subscales confirming that the two politicians were perceived very similarly as far as their personality was

Discussion

The present results suggest that social group membership, as defined by shared political attitude, influences facial mimicry. Specifically, although Bouchard's supporters showed congruent muscle activity to his anger displays, they showed no clear muscle pattern while judging Charest's anger displays. For happiness displays, however, participants showed equivalent mimicry for both Bouchard and Charest's displays. This finding is congruent with the notion that happiness, because it incurs low

Study 2

Study 1 supports the notion that mimicry can be influenced by aspects of the social context, in this case, cues relevant to the shared group status of observer and target. Also, the findings suggest that mimicking different emotions might entrain different costs, leading to variation in the level of mimicry shown. However, in Study 1, the ingroup and outgroup member were two different people, thus it is possible that some of the effects were particular to the individuals in question, as

Participants

Sixty French Canadian men (with a mean age of 23.5 years) participated individually. Half the participants were active basketball players recruited from sports centers at the University of Quebec at Montreal and nearby Colleges, the other half of the participants, also recruited from the University of Quebec at Montreal and nearby Colleges, did neither play basketball themselves nor were basketball fans.

Facial stimuli

The facial stimuli consisted of expressions of happiness, sadness and anger by four

Judgment data

Following the presentation of each stimulus, subjects were asked to assess the emotions portrayed as well as their intensities using an emotion profile. These data will not be discussed in the context of this article.

Facial EMG

The same procedure as in Study 1 was employed with the exception that activity of the Levator Labii Aleaque Nasii, the muscle that lifts the lip in a scowl, was also recorded.2

Manipulation check

Participants were chosen based on whether they were member of a basketball team or not. However, to make sure that non-basketball players were not hardcore basketball fans, and thus would highly identify themselves with basketball players, we assessed participants’ level of identification with basketball, using a modified version of the inclusion-in-the-self scale (Aron et al., 1992). The validity of this scale for ingroup identification has been established by Tropp and Wright (2001). Results

Discussion

In sum, the results underline the impact of the relationship between expresser and observer for facial emotional mimicry. Further, we found that men did not mimic unknown angry men but did mimic other emotions displayed by men. This finding replicates findings by Bourgeois and Hess (2007) who found this effect for both sexes. Yet the finding contrasts with previous studies that found mimicry of anger expressions (e.g., Dimberg, 1982, Dimberg, 1988). However, those results were obtained in

Conclusions

In sum, the results from both studies suggest that emotional facial mimicry is affected by the relationship between the expresser and the observer as well as by the type of emotion expression that is shown. In both studies, negative emotion expressions were mimicked only when shown by an ingroup member; in contrast, happiness was always mimicked.

As mentioned above, the function of mimicry is to increase rapport between interaction partners (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999, see also Hess et al., 1999

Acknowledgements

This study is a part of the first author's doctoral dissertation conducted under the supervision of the second author. The authors wish to thank Nathalie Beaulieu for her contribution to the second study.

References (66)

  • K. Chisholm et al.

    Verbal and facial measures of children's emotion and empathy

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1995)
  • U. Dimberg

    Facial expressions as excitatory and inhibitory stimuli for conditioned autonomic responses

    Biological Psychology

    (1986)
  • U. Dimberg

    Facial reactions, autonomic activity and experienced emotion: a three component model of emotional conditioning

    Biological Psychology

    (1987)
  • H. Aarts et al.

    The silence of the library: environment, situational norm, and social behavior

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2003)
  • H. Aarts et al.

    Automatic normative behavior in environments: the moderating role of conformity in activating situational norms

    Social Cognition

    (2003)
  • A. Aron et al.

    Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1992)
  • J. Aronoff et al.

    The recognition of threatening facial stimuli

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1988)
  • J. Aronoff et al.

    Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1992)
  • J.A. Bargh

    Bypassing the will: towards demystifying behavioral priming effects

  • J.A. Bargh et al.

    Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • J.B. Bavelas et al.

    Form and function in motor mimicry: topographic evidence that the primary function is communicative

    Human Communication Research

    (1988)
  • J.B. Bavelas et al.

    Experimental methods for studying “elementary motor mimicry”

    Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

    (1986)
  • J.B. Bavelas et al.

    “I show how you feel”: motor mimicry as a communicative act

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • M.G. Beaupré et al.

    Cross-cultural emotion recognition among Canadian ethnic groups

    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

    (2005)
  • M.S. Berger et al.

    Some effects of a model's performance on an observer's electromyographic activity

    American Journal of Psychology

    (1975)
  • D.S. Berry et al.

    Some components and consequences of a babyface

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1985)
  • S. Blairy et al.

    Mimicry and the judgment of emotional facial expressions

    Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

    (1999)
  • Bourgeois, P., Hess, U. (2007). Mimicking or not mimicking – is there a choice? The influence of social variables on...
  • M.B. Brewer et al.

    Intergroup relations

  • T.L. Chartrand et al.

    The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1999)
  • T.L. Chartrand et al.

    Beyond the perception–behavior link: the ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry

  • V. Christophe et al.

    Exposure to the social sharing of emotion: emotional impact, listener responses and secondary social sharing

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (1997)
  • A. Dijksterhaus et al.

    The perception–behavior expressway: automatic effects of social perception on social behavior

  • U. Dimberg

    Facial reactions to facial expressions

    Psychophysiology

    (1982)
  • U. Dimberg

    Facial expressions and emotional reactions: a psychobiological analysis of human social behavior

  • U. Dimberg

    Facial electromyography and emotional reactions

    Psychophysiology

    (1990)
  • U. Dimberg et al.

    Behold the wrath: psychophysiological responses to facial stimuli

    Motivation and Emotion

    (1996)
  • U. Dimberg et al.

    Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions

    Psychological Science

    (2000)
  • U. Dimberg et al.

    Facial reactions to emotional stimuli: automatically controlled emotional responses

    Cognition and Emotion

    (2002)
  • N. Ensari et al.

    Negative affect and political sensitivity in crossed categorization: self-reports versus EMG

    Group Processes and Intergroup Relations

    (2004)
  • A.J. Fridlund et al.

    Guidelines for human electromyographic research

    Psychophysiology

    (1986)
  • A.J. Fridlund et al.

    Approaches to Goldie: a field study of human approach responses to canine juvenescence

    Anthrozoos

    (1998)
  • N. Frijda

    The Emotions

    (1986)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text