Attentional influence on the P50 component of the auditory event-related brain potential

https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(92)90011-YGet rights and content

Abstract

To determine if attentional factors influence the suppression of the auditory P50 in a conditioning-testing paradigm, known as the ‘sensory gating’ effect, we tested 10 healthy young adults in four experimental conditions. The first condition was the traditional passive conditioning-testing paradigm in which a pair of identical auditory clicks is administered at an interstimulus interval fixed at 500 ms. The effect of interest is a reduction of P50 amplitude in response to the second stimulus. In the next condition, the second stimulus could be one of two possible frequencies and subjects were required to count one and to ignore the other. The third and fourth experimental conditions involved a motor response. In the third condition, subjects were required to make a unimanual button press to the occurrence of the second stimulus. In the fourth condition, subjects were required to discriminate among two possible second stimuli, and make a unimanual button press to the occurrence of the designated stimulus. Subjects also completed four matched blocks of single stimulus (i.e., unpaired) presentations to provide a baseline for assessing the effect of the warning stimulus on the evoked response. We found, in agreement with previous results, that passive exposure to the paired stimuli produced a suppression of P50 amplitude to the second stimulus. However, we also found that suppression of P50 amplitude was not evident when subjects selectively counted the designated stimuli, and was reduced in magnitude when a simple motor response was required and when a selective motor response was based on stimulus discrimination. In addition, we observed that the amplitude of the P50 was larger with unpaired single stimuli than it was either with the first or second stimulus of a pair, regardless of processing demands. Furthermore, variations in processing demands did not affect P50 amplitude when a single stimulus was presented. This pattern of results suggests that the ‘sensory gating’ effect is not a simple ‘hard-wired’ inhibitory phenomenon. Rather, it may be one manifestation of an attention regulation process that is activated by a warning stimulus and has either inhibitory or excitatory effects on neural transmission, determined by variations in processing demands. Presentation of a warning stimulus may have an additional, unselective suppressing effect, operating independently of this attention regulating process.

References (31)

  • F.C. Donders

    On the speed of mental processes

    Attention and Performance

    (1868)

    Acta Psychol.

    (1969)
  • R. Freedman et al.

    Neurobiological studies of sensory gating in schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (1987)
  • R. Freedman et al.

    Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in inhibitory pathways in schizophrenia: Comparison of medicated and drug free patients

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (1983)
  • G. Gratton et al.

    A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1983)
  • E. Heffley et al.

    Pearl II: Portable laboratory computer system for psychophysiological assessment using event related brain potentials

    Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol.

    (1985)
  • Cited by (88)

    • Neural modulations in the auditory cortex during internal and external attention tasks: A single-patient intracranial recording study

      2022, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      In non-invasive, scalp EEG/MEG recordings, the most reliable finding has been that the N100 − an auditory ERP component that peaks at about 100 msec from stimulus onset (Näätänen & Picton, 1987; Pratt, 2011) and likely has its primary source generator in the auditory cortex (Giard et al., 2000; Godey et al., 2001; Näätänen & Picton, 1987; Neelon et al., 2006; Vaughan & Ritter, 1970) − might be enhanced by attentional focus (Hackley et al., 1990; Hansen and Hillyard, 1980, 1983; Hillyard et al., 1973; Luck & Kappenman, 2012; Näätänen et al., 1981; Rosburg et al., 2009; Woldorff et al., 1993; Woldorff & Hillyard, 1991). In addition to N100, there were some reports that attention-driven enhancement can already be found for the P50 auditory ERP component (Guterman et al., 1992; Wan et al., 2008; Woldorff et al., 1993; Woldorff & Hillyard, 1991; Zabelina et al., 2015) − the component that peaks at about 50 msec from stimulus onset (for review: Pratt, 2011) and also likely originates in the auditory cortex (Huotilainen et al., 1998; Liegeois-Chauvel et al., 1994; Ponton et al., 2002; Pratt, 2011). Finally, several intracranial local field potential (LFP) studies reported enhancement in the auditory cortex as a result of selective attention (Bidet-Caulet et al., 2007; Mesgarani & Chang, 2012; Zion Golumbic et al., 2013).

    • Auditory evoked brain potentials as markers of chronic effects of mild traumatic brain injury in mid-life

      2021, Clinical Neurophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      MCI/AD does not affect this region: Changes to auditory cortical potentials observed for this condition are thought to be mediated by disinhibition of sensory cortex resulting from frontal dysfunction (Golob et al., 2002). Additionally, mechanisms of auditory gating known to affect P50 amplitude are affected by fronto-parietally-mediated processes of attention (Guterman et al., 1992) and the P50 has a prominent frontal contributing source (Knott et al., 2009). However, other neural contributors to auditory sensory potentials may be impacted by mTBI including temporal, limbic, thalamic and midbrain areas.

    • Disruption of Conscious Access in Schizophrenia

      2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text