Skip to main content
Log in

Emotional Responses to Music: Interactive Effects of Mode, Texture, and Tempo

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although much research has explored emotional responses to music using single musical elements, none has explored the interactive effects of mode, texture, and tempo in a single experiment. To this end, a 2 (mode: major vs. minor) × 2 (texture: nonharmonized vs. harmonized) × 3 (tempo: 72, 108, 144 beats per min) within-participants experimental design was employed, in which 177 college students rated four, brief musical phrases on continuous happy-sad scales. Major keys, nonharmonized melodies, and faster tempos were associated with happier responses, whereas their respective opposites were associated with sadder responses. These effects were also interactive, such that the typically positive association between tempo and happiness was inverted among minor, nonharmonized phrases. Some of these effects were moderated by the gender and amount of musical experience of participants. A principal components analysis of responses to the stimuli revealed one negatively and one positively valenced factor of emotional musical stimuli.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Balkwill, L.-L., & Thompson, W. F. (1999). A cross-cultural investigation of the perception of emotion in music: Psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Perception, 17, 43–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Bernston, G. C. (1997). Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1984). Perception of the major/minor distinction: I. Historical and theoretical foundations. Psychomusicology, 4, 3–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalla Bella, S., Peretz, I., Rousseau, L., & Gosselin, N. (2001). A developmental study of the affective value of tempo and mode in music. Cognition, 80, B1–B10.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gabrielsson, A., & Lindström, E. (2001). The influence of musical structure on emotional expression. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 223–248). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagnon, L., & Peretz, I. (2003). Mode and tempo relative contributions to “happy-sad” judgments in equitone melodies. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 25–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garardi, G. M., & Gerken, L. (1995). The development of affective response to modality and melodic contour. Music Perception, 12, 279–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, A., Worral, L., & Sarge, A. (1996). The development of emotional responses to music in young children. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 341–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gundlach, R. H. (1935). Factors determining the characterization of musical phrases. American Journal of Psychology, 47, 624–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinlein, C. P. (1928). The affective characters of the major and minor modes in music. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 101–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henkin, R. I. (1957). The prediction of behavior response patterns to music. The Journal of Psychology, 44, 111–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hevner, K. (1935). The affective character of the major and minor modes in music. American Journal of Psychology, 47, 103–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hevner, K. (1936). Experimental studies of the elements of expression in music. American Journal of Psychology, 48, 246–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hevner, K. (1937). The affective value of pitch and tempo in music. American Journal of Psychology, 49, 621–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husain, G., Thompson, W. F., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2002). Effects of musical tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities. Music Perception, 20, 151–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonas, K., Broemer, P., & Diehl, M. (2000). Attitudinal ambivalence. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 11). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P. N. (1997). Emotional communication in music performance: A functionalist perspective and some data. Music Perception, 14, 383–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 770–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kastner, M. P., & Crowder, R. G. (1990). Perception of the major/minor distinction: IV. Emotional connotations in young children. Music Perception, 8, 189–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellaris, J. J., & Rice, R. C. (1993). The influence of tempo, loudness, and gender of listener on responses to music. Psychology and Marketing, 10, 15–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kivy, P. (1990). Music alone: Philosophical reflections on the purely musical experience. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl, C. L. (2002). Music: A link between cognition and emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 45–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundin, R. W. (1967). An objective psychology of music (2nd ed.). New York: Ronald.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., & Bouchard, B. (1998). Music and emotion: Perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain damage. Cognition, 68, 111–141.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rigg, M. G. (1940). Speed as a determiner of musical mood. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 566–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rigg, M. G. (1964). The mood effects of music: A comparison of data from four investigators. The Journal of Psychology, 58, 427–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robazza, C., Macaluso, C., & D’Urso, V. (1994). Emotional reactions to music by gender, age, and expertise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 939–944.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, E. G., Krysciak, A. M., & Campbell, R. J. (2000). Perceiving emotion in melody: Interactive effects of pitch and rhythm. Music Perception, 18, 155–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., & Oshinsky, J. S. (1977). Cue utilization in emotion attribution from auditory stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 331–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2001). Pleasure, displeasure, and mixed feelings: Are semantic opposites mutually exclusive? Cognition and Emotion, 15, 81–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloboda, J. A. (1991). Music structure and emotional response: Some empirical findings. Psychology of Music, 19, 110–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sopchak, A. L. (1955). Individual differences in responses to different types of music in relation to sex, mood, and other variables. Psychological Monographs, 69, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz, H. L. F. (1895). On the sensation of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music (4th ed.) (A. J. Ellis, Trans.). New York: Longman, Green, and Co. (Original work published 1877)

  • Watson, K. B. (1942). The nature and measurement of musical meanings. Psychological Monographs, 54, 1–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wedin, L. (1972). A multi-dimensional study of perceptual-emotional qualities in music. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 13, 241–257.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gregory D. Webster.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Webster, G.D., Weir, C.G. Emotional Responses to Music: Interactive Effects of Mode, Texture, and Tempo. Motiv Emot 29, 19–39 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-005-4414-0

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-005-4414-0

Key words

Navigation