Skip to main content

Volitional Mediators of Cognition-Behavior Consistency: Self-Regulatory Processes and Action Versus State Orientation

  • Chapter
Action Control

Part of the book series: SSSP Springer Series in Social Psychology ((SSSOC))

Abstract

One of the most striking discrepancies between everyday experience and psychological theorizing concerns the complexity of motivational states. While most psychologists tend to focus on a single behavioral domain (e.g., achievement, affiliation, eating, learning, problemsolving, sex, etc.), we know from everyday experience that people very rarely seem to have just one behavioral inclination in a given situation. In everyday life people usually experience several motivational tendencies simultaneously and more often than not have multiple commitments to a variety of goals. At first glance our task — to explain and predict which of the competing action tendencies a person actually will implement in a given situation — seems to boil down to the objective of establishing the dominant (i. e., strongest) action tendency among all the competing tendencies (e. g., Atkinson & Birch, 1970).

This Chapter was written during the author’s term as a Fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, Stanford, California. It is a pleasure to acknowledge helpful comments on an earlier draft by Jürgen Beckmann and Torgrim Gjesme.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, J.D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament. Leipzig, East Germany: Quelle & Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens. In E. Abderhalden (Ed.), Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden. Bd. VI. Berlin: Urban & Schwarzenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1973). Attitudinal and normative variables as predictors of specific behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 41–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B. (1982). The role of perceptions and attributions for response-outcome contingency in learned helplessness: A commentary and discussion. Journal of Personality, 50, 443–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J.R., & Bower, G.H. (1973). Human associative memory. Washington: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1958). Motives in fantasy, action, and society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W., & Feather, N.T. (1966). A theory of achievement motivation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckmann, J., & Kuhl, J. (1984). Altering information to gain action control: Functional aspects of human information-processing in decision-making. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 224–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D.E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blankenship, V. (1982). The relationship between consummatory value of success and achievement-task difficulty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 901–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T. H., & Bacharach, V. R. (1976). Perceptual tuning and conscious attention: Systems of input regulation in visual information processing. Cognition, 4, 281-302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E.L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörner, D., Kreuzig, H. W., Reither, F., & Stäudel, T. (Eds.). (1983). Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Düker, H. (1983). Über unterschwelliges Wollen. Göttingen, FRG: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on the utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eckblad, G. (1981). Scheme theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, N. S., & Magnusson, D. (Eds.). (1976). Interactional psychology and personality. Toronto: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankel, A., & Snyder, M. L. (1978). Poor performance following unsolvable problems: Learned helplessness or egotism? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1415–1423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, D., & Lepper, M. R. (1974). Effects of extrinsic rewards on children’s subsequent intrinsic interest. Child Development, 45, 1141–1145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grosse, B. (1982). Verhaltenskorrelate von Handlungs-und Lageorientierung: Rehabilität und Validität der Handlungskontroll-Skala (unpublished thesis). Ruhr-University Bochum, FRG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 283–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiroto, D.W., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Generality of learned helplessness in man. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 311–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, M. S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation, and performance: A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological Review 91, 153–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1974). The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koller, P. S., & Kaplan, R. M. (1978). A two-process theory of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1177–1183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. (1975). The endogenous-exogenous partition in attribution theory. Psychological Review, 82, 387–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1977). Meß-und prozeßtheoretische Analysen einiger Person-und Situationsparameter der Leistungsmotivation. Bonn: Bouvier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state versus action orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 155–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1982 a). The expectancy-value approach in the theory of social motivation: Elaborations, extensions, critique. In N.T. Feather (Ed.), Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1982 b). Action vs. state orientation as a mediator between motivation and action. In W. Hacker, W. Volpert, & M. von Cranach (Eds.), Cognitive and motivational aspects of action. Amsterdam: North-Holland-Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1982 c). Handlungskontrolle als metakognitiver Vermittler zwischen Intention und Handeln: Freizeitaktivitäten bei Hauptschülern. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 14, 141–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983 a). Motivation, Konflikt und Handlungskontrolle. Heidelberg, FRG: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983 b). Motivationstheoretische Aspekte der Depressionsgenese: Der Einfluß von Lageorientierung auf Schmerzempfinden, Medikamentenkonsum und Handlungskontrolle. In M. Wolfersdorf, R. Straub, & G. Hole (Eds.), Der depressiv Kranke in der psychiatrischen Klinik: Theorie und Praxis der Diagnostik und Therapie. Regensburg: Roderer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983 c). Emotion, Kognition und Motivation: II. Die funktionale Bedeutung der Emotionen für das problemlösende Denken und für das konkrete Handeln. Sprache und Kognition, 4, 228–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1984). Volitional aspects of achievement motivation and learned helplessness: Toward a comprehensive theory of action control. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in Experimental Personality Research (Vol.13), (pp.99–171). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (in press). Motivation and information processing: A new look at decision-making, dynamic conflict, and action control. In R. M. Sorrentio & E.T. Higgins (Eds.). The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1983). Handlungskontrolle und Umfang der Informationsverarbeitung: Wahl einer vereinfachten (nicht optimalen) Entscheidungsregel zugunsten rascher Handlungsbereitschaft. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 14, 241–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Eisenbeiser, T. (in press). Mediating versus meditating cognitions in human motivation: Action control, inertial motivation, and the alienation effect. In J. Kuhl & J. W. Atkinson (Eds.), Motivation, thought, and action. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Geiger, E. (in press). The dynamic theory of the anxiety-behavior relation: A study on resistance and time allocation. In J. Kuhl & J.W. Atkinson (Eds.), Motivation, thought, and action. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Helle, P. (1984). Motivational and volitional determinants of depression: The degen-erated-intention hypothesis. (Submitted). Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, FRG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Melendez, J. (1984). Undermining intrinsic motivation: Individual differences and duration of exposure to extrinsic rewards (Manuscript in preparation). Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, FRG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Wassiljew, I. (1983). Intrinsic task-involvement coping with failure, and problem-solving: Motivational and emotional determinants of the complexity of action plans (Manuscript submitted for publication). Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, FRG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Weiß, M. (1983). Performance deficits following uncontrollable failure: Impaired action control or global attributions and generalized expectancy deficits? (Manuscript submitted for publication.) Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, FRG.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Berge, D. (1973). Attention and the measurement of perceptual learning. Memory and Cognition, 1, 268–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavelle, T.L., Metalsky, G.I., & Coyne, J.C. (1978). Learned helplessness, test anxiety, and acknowledgment of contingencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 381–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S., & Averill, J. R. (1972). Emotion and cognition: With special reference to anxiety. In Ch. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol.2). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M.R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R.E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the overjustification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindworsky, J. (1923). Der Wille. Seine Erscheinung und Beherrschung nach den Ergebnissen der experimentellen Forschung. Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Sarason, S. B. (1952). A study of anxiety and learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 166–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1981). Metacognition and the rules of delay. In J. Flavell & C. Ross (Eds.), Cognitive and social development: Frontier and possible futures. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. G., & Zeiss, A. R. (1972). Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 204–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, H.N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children’s knowledge of self-control strategies. Child Development, 54, 226–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, M. (1981). The overjustification effect: A developmental test of self-perception interpretations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 809–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, M. (1983). Decrements in intrinsic motivation among rewarded and observer subjects. Child Development, 54, 636–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, L.W., & Liebert, R.M. (1970). Relationship of cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety to physiological arousal and academic performance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35, 332–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neely, H.N. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibition-less spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H.A. (1972). Human problem-solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D., & Rumelhart A.D (1975). Explorations in cognition. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, W. F., & Curtis, N. (1982). Learned helplessness: Not dependent upon cognitions, attributions, or other such phenomenal experiences. Journal of Personality, 50, 387–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D.R., & Astington, J. W. (1983). Children’s acquisition of metalinguistic and metacogni-tive verbs. (Manuscript submitted for publication.) Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Toronto: Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in the child. New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, S., & Sushinsky, L. W. (1975). Overjustification, competing responses, and the acquisition of intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 1116–1125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, S. (1980). A revised model of learned helplessness in humans. Journal of Personality, 48, 103–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, S., & Kubal, L. (1975). The effect of noncontingent reinforcement on tasks of differing importance: Facilitation and learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 680–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligmann, M.E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoresen, C.E., & Mahoney, M.J. (1974). Behavioral self-control. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wine, J.D. (1982). Evaluation anxiety: A cognitive-attentional construct. In H. W. Krohne & L. C. Laux (Eds.), Achievement, stress, and anxiety. Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R.S., Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental psychology. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional Mediators of Cognition-Behavior Consistency: Self-Regulatory Processes and Action Versus State Orientation. In: Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J. (eds) Action Control. SSSP Springer Series in Social Psychology. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-69746-3_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-69746-3_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-69748-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-69746-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics