Skip to main content

Abstract

One of the earliest studies of memory (Ebbinghaus, 1895) showed that list memory, when plotted according to the list’s serial position, was U-shaped. Memory was best at the beginning (primacy effect) and the end (recency effect) of the list, and poorest in the middle. In the late ‘50s, researchers showed that the recency effect, thought to be the short-term memory (STM) component, could be selectively eliminated from the primacy effect and middle portion, thought to be the long-term memory (LTM) component, by delaying recall and filling the delay with distracter activity (e.g., Brown, 1958; Peterson and Peterson, 1959). This manipulation was the beginning of the modern study of short-term memory and helped bring about the so-called cognitive revolution. There has been considerable emphasis on the discovery of dissociable memory processes and development of theories to explain them. Some memory processes (e.g., language based) may be unique to humans, but general theories of memory should also apply, at least in part, to animals as well as humans.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Atkinson, R. C. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89–105). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beecher, M., Petersen, M., Zoloth, S., Moody, D., & Stebbins, W. (1979). Perception of conspecific vocalizations by Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscatd). Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 16, 443–460.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bolhuis, J. J., & van Kampen, H. S. (1988). Serial position curves in spatial memory of rats: primacy and recency effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 135–149.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Briggs, G. E. (1954). Acquisition, extinction, and recovery functions in retroactive inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 285–293.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, J. P, Gill, T. V., & Braggio, J. T. (1981). Serial Position and clustering effects in chimpanzee’s “free recall.” Memory and Cognition, 9, 651–660.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. (1958). Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 12–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R. & Dodd, B. (1980). Hearing by eye. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 85–99.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Castro, C. A., & Larsen, T. (1992). Primacy and recency effects in nonhuman primates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 18, 335–340.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ceraso, J., & Henderson, A. (1966). Unavailability and associative loss in RI and PI: Second try. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 314–316.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, R. G., Wright, A. A., & Sands, S. F. (1991). Interstimulus interval and viewing time effects in monkey list memory. Animal Learning & Behavior, 19, 153–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowey, A. (1968). Discrimination. In L Weiskrantz (Ed.) Analysis of behavioral change (pp. 189–238). NY: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1983). The purity of auditory memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Section B, 302B, 251–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1986). Auditory and temporal factors in the modality effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2, 268–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. & Morton J. (1969). Precategorical acoustic storage (PAS). Perception & Psychophysics, 5, 365–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato, M. R. (1973). Delayed matching and short-term memory in monkeys. In G. H. Bower (Ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Theory and Research, 7, New York: Academic Press, pp. 227–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato, M. R. & Salmon, D. P. (1982). Tune discrimination in monkeys (Cebus apella) and rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 10, 126–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato, M. R., Salmon, D. P., & Colombo, M. (1985). Extent and limits of the matching concept in monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 35–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R. T., & Fitts, S. S. (1976). Memory and coding processes in discrimination learning, In D. L. Medin, W. A. Roberts, & R. T. Davis (Eds.), Processes of Animal Memory. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devine, J. V., & Jones, L. C. (1975). Matching-to-successive samples: A multiple-unit memory task with rhesus monkeys. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 7, 438–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewson, J. H., III & Cowey, A. (1969). Discrimination of auditory sequences by monkeys. Nature, 222, 695–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dewson, J. H., III, Wertheim, G. A., & Lynch, J. C. (1968). Acquisition of successive auditory discrimination in monkeys. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 43, 162–163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. E. (1902). Grundzuge der Psychologie. Leipzig: Von Viet, 1902.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, D. R. (1973). Memory processing in Macaca speciosa: Mental processes revealed by reaction time experiments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Carnegie-Mellon University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. & Mobley, L. A. (1976). The modality effect: What happens in long-term memory? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 519–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forrester, W. E. (1970). Retroactive inhibition and spontaneous recovery in the A-B, D-C paradigm. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 525–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaffan, D. (1977). Recognition memory after short retention intervals in fornix-transected monkeys. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 577–588.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, J. M., Thompson, C. P. & Maskarinec, A. S. (1974). Negative recency in initial free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 71–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Gardiner, J. M, & Gregg, V. H. (1982). Modality and phonological similarity effects in serial recall: Does one’s own voice play a role? Memory & Cognition, 10, 176–180.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glanzer, M. (1972). Storage mechanisms in recall. In G.H. Bower and J. T. Spence (Eds.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. (Vol. 5, pp. 129–193) NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 351–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Bradley, M. M., Kraus, T.A., & Renzaglia, G. J. (1983). Studies of the long-term recency effect: Support for a contextually guided retrieval hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 231–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Bradley, M. M., Stevenson, J. A., Kraus, T. A., Tkachuk, M. J, Gretz, A. L., Fish, J. H., & Turpin, B. M. (1980). A Two-process account of long-term serial position effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 6, 355–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, S. & Marier, P. (1979). The analysis of animal communication. In Marier, P. & Vandenberg, J. G. (Eds.) Social Behavior and Communication, Vol. 3, New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillund, G. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 19, 1–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harper, D. N, McLean, A. P., & Dalrymple-Alford, J. C. (1993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 307.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, J. M., Downey, P., Iverson, S. D. & Pratt, S. R. (1977). Control of responding by location of auditory stimuli: Adjacency of sound and response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 28, 243–251.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace model. Psychological Review, 95, 528–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, M. S., Pike, R., Bain, J. D., & Tehan, G. (1989). Global matching: A comparison of the SAM, Minerva II, Matrix, and TOD AM models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 33, 36–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keppel, G. & Underwood, B. J. (1962). Proactive inhibition in short-term retention of single items. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 153–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kesner, R. P. & Novak, J. M. (1982). Serial position curve in rats: role of the dorsal hippocampus. Science, 218, 173–175.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koppenaal, R. J. (1963). Time changes in the strengths of A-B, A-C lists: Spontaneous recovery. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 310–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korsnes, M. S. (1995). Retention intervals and serial list memory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80, 723–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korsnes, M. S. & Gilinsky, S. A. (1993). Aging and serial list picture memory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 1011–1014.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M. & Cohen N. J. (1989). Catastrophic interference in connectionist networks: The sequential learning problem. In G.H. Bower (Ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 24, pp. 109–165). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melton, A. W. & Irwin, J. M. (1940). The influence of degree of interpolated learning on retroactive inhibition and the overt transfer of specific responses. American Journal of Psychology, 53, 173–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mensink, G. & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (1988). A model for interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe-Eich, J. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model. Psychological Review, 89, 627–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Modigliani, V. & Hedges, D. G. (1987). Distributed rehearsals and the primacy effect in single-trial free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 13, 426–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., Marcus, S. M. & Ottley P. (1981). The acoustic correlates of “speechlike”: A use of the suffix effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 568–593.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B. (1982). A theory for the storage and retrieval of item and associative information. Psychological Review, 89, 609–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B., Jr. & Kahana M. J. (1993). List-strength and list-length effects: Reply to Shiffrin, Ratcliff, Murnane and Nobel (1993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1450–1453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neath, I. (1993). Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition. Memory & Cognition, 21, 689–698.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Neath, I., & Knoedler, A. J. (1994). Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 776–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neiworth, J. J. & Wright, A. A. (1994). Monkeys (Macaca mulatto) learn category matching in a nonidentical same/different task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 429–435.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, L. R. & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. & Konick, A. W. (1966). On the role of interference in short-term retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 221–231.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L., & Phillips, L. (1965). Short-term temporal changes in free-recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 132–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L., Stark, K. & Fraser, J. (1968). Temporal changes in interference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 672–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L., Stark, K. & Henschel, D. (1969). Conditions of recovery after unlearning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, (1, Pt. 2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, P., Chih-Ta, T., Aggleton, J. P. & Rawlins, J. N. P. (1991). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, J. T. E. (1979). Precategorical acoustic storage and postcategorical lexical storage. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 265–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, W. A. & Kraemer, P. J. (1981). Recognition memory for lists of visual stimuli in monkeys and humans. Animal Learning and Behavior, 9, 587–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. Ill & Crowder, R. G. (1975). Spacing of lists in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 590–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. & Williams R. J. (1986). Learning internal representations by error propagation. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland & the PDP Research Group (Eds.) Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations, (pp. 318–362). Cambridge, MA: Bradford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 63–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sands, S. F. & Wright, A. A. (1980a). Primate memory: retention of serial list items by a rhesus monkey. Science, 209, 938–940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sands, S. F. & Wright (1980b). Serial probe recognition performance by a rhesus monkey and a human with 10- and 20- item lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 6, 386–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. & Marier, P. (1980). Monkey responses to different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210, 801–803.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shand, M. A & Klima, E. S. (1981). Nonauditory suffix effects in congenitally deaf signers of American Sign Language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 464–474.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, K. L., Jacobs, W. J., & LoLordo, V. M. (1980). Stimulus-reinforcer interactions in Pavlovian conditioning of pigeons: Implications for selective associations. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 586–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Steyvers, M. (1997). A model for recognition memory: REM—retrieving effectively from memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 145–166.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, H. G. & Martin, E. (1970). Effects of response set similarity on unlearning and spontaneous recovery. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 230–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silverstein, A. (1967). Unlearning, spontaneous recovery, and the partial-reinforcement effect in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 15–21.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Slamecka, N. J. (1966). A search for spontaneous recovery of verbal associations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 205–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1992). Reducing interference in distributed memories through episodic gating, In A. F. Healy, S. M. Kosslyn & R. M. Shiffrin (Eds.). From learning theory to connectionist theory: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 1. pp. 227–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stebbins, W. C. (1970). Studies of hearing and hearing loss in the monkey. In W. C. Stebbins (Ed.) Animal Psychophysics (pp. 41-66). NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stebbins, W. C. (1971). Hearing. In A. M. Schrier & F. Stollnitz (Eds.) Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. 3, pp. 159–192). NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stebbins, W. C. (1973). Hearing of old world monkeys. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 38, 357–364.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Surprenant, A. M., Pitt, M. A. & Crowder, R. G. (1993). Auditory recency in immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 193–223.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. K. R. (1980). Auditory cued reversal and matching-to-sample learning by rhesus monkeys. Antropologia Comtemporanea, 3 284–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. K. R. & Herman, L. M. (1977). Science, 195, 501.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, B. J. (1948a). Retroactive and proactive inhibition after five and forty-eight hours. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, B. J. (1948b). “Spontaneous recovery” of verbal associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 429–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, W. Jr. and Greene, S. L. (1984). Acquisition of absolute discriminations in pigeons. In: M. L. Commons & R. J. Herrnstein (Eds.) Quantitative Analysis of Behavior Vol. IV pp. 231–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waser, P. M. (1975). Experimental playbacks show vocal mediation if intergroup avoidance in forest monkeys. Nature, 255, 56–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waser, P. M. (1977). Sound localization by monkeys; A field experiment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2, 427–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waser, P. M. & Waser, M. S. (1977). Experimental studies of primate vocalization: Specializations for long-distance propagation. Zietzchrift fur Tierpsychology, 43, 239–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waugh, N. C. & Norman, D. A. (1965). Primacy memory. Psychological Review, 12, 89–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, J. G. (1964). Auditory discrimination behavior of normal monkeys. Journal of Auditory Research, 4, 81–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingfield, A. & Byrnes, D. L. (1972). Decay of information in short-term memory. Science, 176, 690–692.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A. (1990). Memory processing by pigeons, monkeys, and people. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 23, pp. 25–70). New York: Academic press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A. (1999). Auditory list memory and interference processes in monkeys: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 25, 284–296.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A., Santiago, H. C, Sands, S. F., Kendrick, D. F. & Cook, R. G. (1985). Memory Processing of serial lists by pigeons, monkeys, and people. Science, 229: 287–289.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A., Shyan M. R. & Jitsumori, M. (1990). Auditory same/different concept learning by monkeys. Animal Learning & Behavior, 18, 287–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A., Urcuioli, P. J., & Sands, S. F. (1986). Proactive interference in animal memory research. In: D. F. Kendrick, M. Rilling & R. Denny (Eds.) Theories of Animal Memory, Lawrence Erlbaum, Englewood Cliffs, N.J., pp. 101–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zoloth, S., Petersen, M., Beecher, M., Green, S., Marler, P., Moody, D. & Stebbins, W. (1979). Species-specific perceptual processing of vocal sounds by old world monkeys. Science, 204, 870–873.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wright, A.A. (2002). Monkey Visual and Auditory Memory. In: Fountain, S.B., Bunsey, M.D., Danks, J.H., McBeath, M.K. (eds) Animal Cognition and Sequential Behavior. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0821-2_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0821-2_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-5255-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-0821-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics